Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285 – 306

Voluntary hyperventilation in the treatment of panic


disorder—functions of hyperventilation, their implications for
breathing training, and recommendations for standardization
Alicia E. Meureta,T, Thomas Ritzb, Frank H. Wilhelmc, Walton T. Rothd
a
Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders at Boston University, United States
b
Psychological Institute III, University of Hamburg, Germany
c
Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Switzerland
d
Stanford University and The Veterans Affairs Palo Alto Health Care System, United States
Received 6 July 2004; received in revised form 18 November 2004; accepted 11 January 2005

Abstract

Hyperventilation has numerous theoretical and empirical links to anxiety and panic. Voluntary hyperventilation
(VH) tests have been applied experimentally to understand psychological and physiological mechanisms that
produce and maintain anxiety, and therapeutically in the treatment of anxiety disorders. From the theoretical
perspective of hyperventilation theories of anxiety, VH is useful diagnostically to the clinician and educationally to
the patient. From the theoretical perspective of cognitive–behavior therapy, VH is a way to expose patients with
panic disorder to sensations associated with panic and to activate catastrophic cognitions that need restructuring.
Here we review panic disorder treatment studies using breathing training that have included VH. We differentiate
the roles of VH in diagnosis, education about symptoms, training of breathing strategies, interoceptive exposure,
and outcome measurement—discussing methodological issues specific to these roles and VH test reliability and
validity. We propose how VH procedures might be standardized in future studies.
D 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Voluntary hyperventilation; Panic disorder; Breathing training; Anxiety; Hypocapnia; pCO2

T Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (A.E. Meuret).

0272-7358/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.01.002
286 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

1. Introduction

Hyperventilation has a long history of being linked to anxiety. Breathing-related symptoms such as
dyspnea,1 shortness of breath, or feelings of suffocation are central features of the experience of panic.
This is reflected in the criteria for panic disorder in the Statistical Manual for Psychiatric Disorders
(DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Additional symptoms on this list are dizziness and
tingling sensations in the extremities, both of which are typically produced by hyperventilation-induced
alkalosis.2 Subjective anxiety has been associated with hyperventilation3 both in individuals suffering
from anxiety disorders and in those who do not (e.g., Rapee & Medoro, 1994; Rapee, Brown, Antony, &
Barlow, 1992). These links have motivated researchers to examine in a variety of experimental subjects
what happens when hypocapnia4 is produced by voluntary hyperventilation (VH) (Antony, Brown, &
Barlow, 1997; Gorman et al., 1994; Holt & Andrews, 1989; Nardi, Valenca, Nascimento, & Zin, 2001;
Papp et al., 1997; Spinhoven, Onstein, Sterk, & le Haen-Versteijnen, 1992; Wilhelm, Gerlach, & Roth,
2001). Two prominent psychophysiological models of panic, the hyperventilation model and the false
suffocation alarm model, have assigned to abnormalities in respiratory gas exchange a central role in the
development of panic and its maintenance (see Roth, Wilhelm & Pettit, in press; Smoller, Pollack, Otto,
Rosenbaum, & Kradin, 1996, for a review).
The hyperventilation model of panic disorder (PD) assumes that at some time in the learning history
of the patient, symptoms elicited by hyperventilation led to fear that in turn fuelled further
hyperventilation, creating a vicious circle spiraling upwards to panic (Ley, 1985). Although it has
been difficult to prove the etiological role of hyperventilation in panic (Roth, Wilhelm, Pettit & Meuret,
in press), a number of findings support its relevance. These include lower basal pCO25 levels (e.g., Papp
et al., 1997; Roth, Wilhelm, & Trabert, 1998; Salkovskis, Jones, & Clark, 1986), stronger psychological
response to VH (e.g., Antony et al., 1997; Gorman et al., 1994; Holt & Andrews, 1989), and slower
recovery from VH (Gorman et al., 1988; Maddock & Carter, 1991; Wilhelm, Alpers, Meuret, & Roth,
2001) in patients suffering from PD compared to healthy controls. Respiratory abnormalities such as
disorganized breathing patterns or frequent sighing have also been observed in individuals with panic
disorder (Abelson, Weg, Nesse, & Curtis, 2001; Martinez et al., 1996; Stein, Millar, Larsen, & Kryger,
1995). A specificity of these abnormalities for panic and not other anxiety disorders was evidenced in
some studies (e.g., Wilhelm et al., 2001; Wilhelm, Trabert, & Roth, 2001a, 2001b).
However, not all results support the hyperventilation model. Hypocapnia can be absent in individuals
with panic disorder during baselines6 (Holt & Andrews, 1989; Woods et al., 1986), and even during
naturally occurring panic attacks (Garssen, Buikhuisen, & van Dyck, 1996; Hibbert & Pilsbury, 1988).
Van den Hout et al. (1992) observed hypocapnia at baseline with further decreases during an exciting

1
Dyspnea is defined as abnormal or uncomfortable breathing in the context of what is normal for a person according to his or her level of
fitness and exertional threshold for breathlessness.
2
Respiratory alkalosis is a condition of excessive alkalinity of arterial blood, most often associated with respiratory (breathing) disorders. The
pH (a measure of acidity or alkalinity) is high and carbon dioxide levels are low.
3
Hyperventilation (overbreathing) is defined as a combination of rate and depth of breathing that is too much for the body’s needs at a
particular point in time.
4
Acute decreases in arterial pCO2.
5
Partial pressure of arterial blood carbon dioxide.
6
Quiet sitting periods.
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 287

film or fearful imagery in both persons with PD and non-PD anxiety disorder patients and these levels
corresponded to levels of subjective distress, as compared to healthy controls. They interpreted the
changes in end-tidal pCO27 as an effect of fear or excitement rather than their cause. A general
conclusion from the experiments cited above is that hyperventilation, if present, is not specific to PD
patients.
The false suffocation alarm model of PD postulates a role for hyperventilation but a different one from
the hyperventilation model. Patients hyperventilate to achieve a persistent hypocapnic state at a safe
distance below the threshold of their overly sensitive biological suffocation alarm (Klein, 1993). While
the hyperventilation theory postulates that anxiety is caused by hypocapnic breathing, Klein’s theory
postulates that hyperventilation serves as a way to avoid the anxiety caused by hypercapnic breathing.
Support for this model comes from studies that showed exaggerated responses of people with PD to
inhalation of gas mixtures with higher than room air CO2 levels (e.g., Gorman et al., 1994; Rapee et al.,
1992) and that found greater sensitivity of chemoreceptors8 in these individuals (e.g., Lousberg, Griez, &
van den Hout, 1988). Some studies, however, have not reported PD-specific hypersensitivities or lower
thresholds for rises in ventilation during rebreathing tests (e.g., Katzman et al., 2002; Schmidt, Telch, &
Jaimez, 1996). A general criticism of these inhalation studies is that the CO2 concentrations given are
higher than those occurring naturally (McNally, Hornig, & Donnell, 1995).
Cognitive and behavioral models of panic also assign a role to hyperventilation. According to them,
symptoms of hypocapnia tend to be misinterpreted and catastrophized by patients with PD as signs of
impending, dire harm. These misinterpretations of bodily sensations lead to a positive feedback loop between
anxiety and physical sensations, eventuating in full-blown panic attacks (Clark, 1986; Reiss & McNally,
1985). In cognitive–behavior therapy, VH serves as a convenient way of exposing individuals with PD to
some of their panic symptoms (interoceptive exposure) and to the activation of catastrophic thoughts.
Given the theoretical interest in the link between hyperventilation and anxiety, it is not surprising that
VH has often been employed in therapies. One of the attractions of respiration as a therapeutic focus is
that, unlike autonomic anxiety responses, respiration is subject to conscious control. VH has the
potential of voluntarily creating a state that is presumed to occur, and is sometimes observed to occur,
without conscious intent, during states of anxiety, panic, and other emotions.
Despite its widespread use in studies of panic disorder, little consensus exists on standards of a VH
test. Patients are typically instructed to breathe faster than normal for a certain period of time, but beyond
this very general feature, studies vary greatly with regard to procedural parameters and outcome
assessment. Fig. 1 shows the progression of a VH challenge performed in our laboratory with a panic
patient (Meuret, Wilhelm, & Roth, 2002). After approximately 1 min, the patient met our prescribed
criteria of lowering pCO2 levels to 20 mm Hg at a respiratory rate of 18 breaths/min. After 3 min of VH,
the patient stopped her efforts at hyperventilation, and her pCO2 began to return to its initial level. Drops
and spikes indicate irregularity in the breathing patterns such as sighs or pauses.
During a VH challenge, patients typically report elevated levels of anxiety accompanied by symptoms
of shortness of breath, dizziness, heart racing, trembling, and tingling or numbness in the extremities.
They also may report feelings of unreality or fear of losing control. These symptoms generally subside

7
Partial pressure of arterial blood carbon dioxide collected from the nose via nasal cannula.
8
Central and peripheral chemoreceptors participate in the control of ventilation by detecting short-term (acute) and long-term (chronic)
alterations in blood gases (po2, pco2, and pH).
288 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

45
During VH Recovery from VH
40

35

30

25

20

15

10

5
RR PCO2
0
16:57

16:58

16:59

17:00

17:01

17:02

17:03

17:04

17:05

17:06

17:07

17:08
Fig. 1. Changes in pCO2 and respiration rate during standardized VH.

by the end of an 8-min recovery period, but in some patients, physical symptoms, anxiety, or feelings of
unreality can persist.

2. Voluntary hyperventilation in panic disorder treatment (example breathing training)

Below we examine ways that VH has been applied clinically in order to examine further its role in
clinical diagnosis, education about symptoms, breathing training, interoceptive exposure, and outcome
measurement. Since most applications of VH are linked to breathing training (BT), we will exemplify
various roles of VH using studies that have focused on testing BT effects. The rationale for conventional
BT is to eliminate persistent or acute decreases in arterial pCO2 by means of slow, diaphragmatic
breathing, and thereby prevent anxious states. This rationale is based on a hyperventilation model of
panic disorder. BT has also been included as a component of cognitive–behavioral treatment packages
(e.g., Craske, Barlow, & Meadows, 2000), often with an interoceptive exposure rationale. In this context,
BT has also come under criticism of being counterproductive, as we will discuss later.
We identified nine published studies with BT as the sole therapeutic modality, or that emphasized BT
in combination with few other components in designs where inferences about its efficacy as a separate
component could be made (Bonn, Readhead, & Timmons, 1984; Clark, Salkovskis, & Chalkley, 1985;
Craske, Rowe, Lewin, & Noriega-Dimitri, 1997; Franklin, 1989; Hibbert & Chan, 1989; Meuret,
Wilhelm, Ritz, & Roth, submitted; Rapee, 1985; Salkovskis et al., 1986; Schmidt et al., 2000) (Table 1).
BT instructions typically emphasized slow and abdominal breathing. Treatment duration ranged from 2
to 4 weeks, with at least one therapist-guided session per week. In the study of Bonn et al. (1984), BT
and in vivo exposure were always given together, while in other studies, BT was integrated into CBT
packages (Craske et al., 1997; Schmidt et al., 2000). All of these studies included VH in one form or
Table 1
Outcome studies of breathing training (BT) for panic disorder
Study BT treatment (n) Type of BT technique Control (n) Type of BT Results for BT
outcome measures
Bonn et al. (1984) 2 sessions/2 weeks Slow and diaphragmatic Exposure Psychological BT in combination with exposure is at
(plus seven weekly breathing only (5) and physiological least as effective as exposure alone but is
sessions of in vivo superior at 6-month follow-up in terms of
exposure) (7) mean resting RR, global phobia score,
somatic symptoms score, and no further
panic attacks

A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306


Rapee (1985) 3 sessions/4 weeks Diaphragmatic breathing – Psychological Decrease in panic attack frequency and
(1) and reduction of respiratory severity, symptoms, and anxiety
rate and habitual sighing
and yawning
Clark et al. 2 sessions/2 week Slow and regular breathing – Psychological Reduction in panic attack frequency and
(1985) (18) supported by audiotape self-reported anxiety and depression after
2 weeks of training.
Salkovskis et al. 4 sessions/4 weeks Slow and regular breathing – Psychological Reduction in panic attack frequency and
(1986) (9) supported by audiotape and physiological self-reported anxiety and depression;
(12 breaths/min) increase of pCO2 to normal levels
Hibbert and Chan 2 sessions/2 weeks Paced breathing supported Placebo Psychological Self-reported measures of anxiety improve
(1989) (plus 3 weeks of by audiotape treatment equally in treatment and placebo groups.
exposure in vivo) (19) At posttreatment. observer but not
(21) self-report ratings indicated a greater
improvement in patients receiving BT
Franklin (1989) 1 session/4 weeks Slow breathing Delayed Psychological BT was superior in reducing panic attack
(compared to IIR, (12 breaths/min) treatment (4) frequency and severity, catastrophic
CM and IR) (4) cognitions, anticipatory anxiety, and
behavioral measures of agoraphobia
Craske et al. 2 sessions/2 weeks Slow and Interoceptive Psychological Interoceptive exposure was more
(1997) (CBT: 12 session/ diaphragmatic breathing exposure efficacious on certain measures than BT in
12 weeks) (18) (20) combination with cognitive restructuring
and in vivo exposure
Schmidt et al. 2 sessions/2 weeks Diaphragmatic breathing Delayed Psychological Active treatment groups improved similarly
(2000) (CBT: 12 session/ treatment to WL on the outcome measures; patients
12 weeks) (21) (24) in the CBT group showed a trend towards
higher end-state functioning and sought
less additional treatment
Meuret et al. 5 sessions/4 weeks Capnometry-assisted BT Delayed Psychological Major clinical severity measures,
(submitted)a (20) targeting slow (13, 11, 9, treatment and physiological respiratory rate, and pCO2 improved
and 6 breaths/min over (17) substantially from pre- to post-therapy and
course of 4 weeks), regular, remained stable or declined further at
diaphragmatic, shallow 12-month FU
breathing patterns
All employed hyperventilation tests.
BT=breathing training, RR=respiratory rate, IIR=in situ isometric relaxation, CM=cognitive modification, IR=imaginal rehearsal, CBT=cognitive–behavioral therapy, WL=waiting list control,
FU=follow-up.

289
a
More details about the methodology of this study can be found in Meuret, Wilhelm, and Roth (2001, 2004).
290 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

another. Surprisingly, although the target of the BT treatment is respiration and thus a physiological one,
few studies measured breathing pattern or gas exchange as outcome variables.
The five controlled studies that added BT to other therapy components offered a mixed picture of the
therapeutic efficacy of BT. While two studies with small patient numbers showed greater efficacy of BT
than control conditions at post-treatment (Franklin, 1989) or long-term assessments (Bonn et al., 1984),
two studies (Craske et al., 1997; Schmidt et al., 2000) with somewhat larger sample sizes did not show
superiority of BT (as added component to CBT or in comparison with interoceptive exposure) at post-
treatment or follow-up assessments. The study of Hibbert and Chan (1989) found a greater efficacy of
BT post-treatment as measured by therapist ratings but not by patients’ self-report. In general, results
from the controlled studies did not show a marked benefit of BT in panic disorder. A more complete
description of these studies and conclusions about the efficacy of BT appears in Meuret, Wilhelm, Ritz,
and Roth (2003). Here we will concern ourselves with technical and conceptual issues regarding the use
of VH in these studies and in BT, in general, and how VH might be best conducted and utilized.

2.1. Voluntary hyperventilation procedures

In the BT studies we reviewed, the VH instructions, verification, and assessment of outcome varied
widely (Table 2). Test durations ranged from 1 to 3 min and target breathing rates from 18 to 60 breaths/
min. The instructed breathing pattern was often described as bfast and deep breathing,Q but also sometimes
as bforced ventilation,Q bvigorous panting,Q or bfast breathing with hard exhalations.Q Sometimes no
specific instructions were reported. In some studies, volume was not mentioned; thus if patients panted
shallowly, they may not have substantially lowered their arterial pCO2, or, paradoxically, may even have
increased it since none of the CO2 deep in the lungs may have been exchanged with outside air. Target
pCO2 levels were specified only in our own study, where patients were required in a 3-min period to
quickly reach a level of 20 mm Hg and maintain it. Checking for compliance of individual patients with test
instructions was reported in only two studies (Bonn et al., 1984; Meuret et al., submitted). Various test
outcome measures were used, although often poorly specified. In some studies, the therapist inquired in a
more or less structured fashion about what symptoms had occurred during hyperventilation and whether
they were similar to the patient’s panic symptoms. Sometimes rating scales were given, the details of which
were not always specified. Results of the VH were reported when VH was used for diagnosis (Hibbert &
Chan, 1989) or outcome assessment (Craske et al., 1997), but not when used for education about breathing
(Franklin, 1989) or interoceptive exposure (Schmidt et al., 2000). Physiological measurements of the
extent of hypocapnia as the indicator of manipulation success were rare. Overall, consensus on technical,
procedural, and outcome variables of VH in BT of PD was lacking, partly because of the various roles the
VH takes on in BT programs, but probably also because adequate instrumentation for measuring the extent
of hypocapnia (capnometers) was considered too cumbersome or expensive.
Although VH is usually conducted in an individual setting, a group setting is also possible, which is
more time-efficient but also has disadvantages. Visual or tactile monitoring of the respiration of
individuals in a group is difficult. The manual of Craske (1998) says that bthe therapist will need to keep
encouraging clients to breathe fast and especially to exhale hardQ (p.32), but unmonitored clients may not
comply with this out of fear of the symptoms and embarrassment about panicking in front of other
people. When VH is used therapeutically, it is easier to respond to individual reactions and idiosyncratic
beliefs about it in one-to-one setting. Data from individuals collected in a group setting cannot be treated
as independent cases statistically, if responses in one patient can influence responses in others.
Table 2
Techniques of hyperventilation used in breathing training studies and other relevant quality criteria
Study Test duration Breathing Target Occurrence in the Check of Outcome criteria Role
pattern breathing rate therapy compliance with
test instruction
Rapee and 1 1/2 min Not reported 25 breaths/min Once prior to Not reported Verbal symptom report Education about
Medoro treatment and judgments of symptoms
(1994) similarity to past PAs
Bonn et al. 3 min Vigorous chest 60 breaths/min Once prior to RR and mode Verbal symptom report Diagnosis; education
(1984) panting though treatment of breathing and judgments of about symptoms

A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306


mouth and was monitored similarity to past PAs
nose
Clark et al. Approximately Fast and deep Not reported Once prior to Not reported Symptom rating sheet Diagnosis; education
(1985) 2 min breathing treatment and questionnaire about symptoms
through mouth about similarity to
past PAs
Salkovskis et al. 2 min Forced Not reported Repeated VH as Not reported Symptom rating sheet Education about
(1986) ventilation part of first session and questionnaire about symptoms
and home work similarity to past PAs
Hibbert and Approximately Fast and deep Not reported Once in BT group Not reported Symptom rating sheet Diagnosis; education
Chan (1989)a 2 min breathing in the first session, and questionnaire about about symptoms;
through mouth in the placebo similarity to past PAs training of breathing
group after the strategies
final assessment
Franklin (1989) Three times Not reported 18 breaths/min Repeated VH as Not reported Hyperventilation Education about
(standing, sitting, part of first session questionnaire symptoms; training
and lying), up to and home training of breathing strategies
1 1/2 min, with
30-s breaks
Craske et al. 1– 1 1/2 min Fast breathing 3 faster than Repeated VH as Not reported Discussion with therapist Education about
(1997)b with hard normal part of breathing about symptoms produced symptoms; interoceptive
exhalations training and and judgment of similarity exposure and extinction;
interoceptive to past PAs outcome measure
exposure
Schmidt et al. 1 min Deep and fast Not reported Repeated VH as Not reported Symptom intensity and Interoceptive exposure
(2000)c breathing part of similarity to past PAs and extinction
interoceptive
exposure
Meuret et al. 3 min Fast and deep 18 breaths/min Repeated VH Instructed Symptom rating and Outcome measure
(submitted) before and after reduction questionnaire about
treatment of pCO2 to 20 similarity to past PAs
mm Hg
a
The authors cite Clark et al. (1985) for the procedure of their VH test.

291
b
Information retrieved from therapy manual (Craske, 1998).
c
Information retrieved from therapy manual (Craske & Barlow, 1990) cited in Telch et al. (1993).
292 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

How participants are prepared for VH is an important procedural variable that has varied between
studies. Bonn et al. (1984) advised that bany suggestion of effects should preferably be avoidedQ and
offered only what they called bminimal description of possible effects (dslight dizziness and tinglingT)Q to
alert their patients (and healthy controls) to unexpected and unfamiliar symptoms. Extensive prior
information describing all conceivable reactions as normal and harmless is likely to reduce anxiety,
which is counterproductive if VH is to be used for exposure. On the other hand, certain prior information
may increase anxiety. Compernolle, Hoogduin, and Joele (1979) warned their hyperventilation
syndrome (HVS) patients that they might experience severe anxiety in order to increase the likelihood
that VH would provoke panic in them, since the investigators felt patients would be most receptive to
explanations of the rationale of therapy immediately after an attack. In any case, a certain amount of
preparatory information is inevitable, since an explanation of the rationale and a discussion of risks are
essential for any ethical therapy procedure. The consent form required by institutional review boards for
therapy in a research context is an important source of preparatory information. Some boards, in trying to
make clients cognizant of all conceivable risks, can make a medically, rather benign, procedure appear
fraught with dangers, and make worriers worry even more.

2.2. Roles of voluntary hyperventilation in breathing training

An examination of what has been written about BT shows that VH has been given a number of
distinct roles: diagnostic test, source of education about symptoms, teacher of breathing strategies,
means of interoceptive exposure and extinction, and measure of outcome. In most studies that we
reviewed, VH assumed more than one role.

2.2.1. Diagnosis
The central goal for BT in panic disorder is the reduction of hypocapnic breathing, but
hyperventilation may not be characteristic of all individuals with PD. Historically, VH was used to
diagnose HVS, a disorder which by definition was caused by repeated or sustained hypocapnic
breathing. Such patients might complain of shortness of breath, light-headedness, chest pain,
palpitations, or sweating, without apparent organic causes. Hypocapnia and disturbance of the acid–
base balance were thought to be the origin of these symptoms (Lum, 1975). Although no general
consensus about diagnostic criteria for HVS was reached, clinicians and researchers often employed a
VH test for determining whether the patient’s symptoms were indeed due to hyperventilation (Bass,
1997). The parameters of the test were rarely standardized (Hornsveld, Garssen, & van Spiegel, 1995),
but in general, patients were instructed to hyperventilate until symptoms occurred for periods from a few
to 30 min, and these symptoms were then compared with the complaints for which patients sought
treatment. Similarity was taken to mean that HVS was indeed the cause of the patient’s distress, in which
case BT would be an appropriate treatment.
With the development of the current American Psychiatry Association classification of anxiety
disorders, which does not include HVS, the suspicion arose that many patients diagnosed as having HVS
would now be diagnosed as having PD. Consequently, VH was applied to patients meeting criteria for
PD in the same way it was to presumed patients with HVS, namely, to identify a subgroup of individuals
whose attacks are associated with hyperventilation and who might respond particularly well to BT. Like
for HVS, this subgroup has usually been considered to be patients who respond to VH with self-reports
of discomfort, anxiety, or panic. Some investigators have treated only patients who responded in this
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 293

way (Bonn et al., 1984; Clark et al., 1985), while others have used VH to identify a subgroup for
assignment to a hyperventilation-oriented treatment module within their study (Hibbert & Chan, 1989).
Bonn et al. (1984) identified treatment candidates with their VH test: Participants had to breathe 60
breaths/min for at least 3 min unless extreme physical symptoms (e.g., dizziness) were experienced. For
immediate relief from symptoms during this procedure, patients were shown how to rebreathe into a bag.
If the symptoms produced were recognized by patients as being similar to those experienced bin the
past,Q they met the diagnostic criteria for being true dhyperventilators.T Only 33% of their patients were
able to complete the 3-min VH, and 20 of 21 met these criteria, whereas 96% of a healthy control group
completed the test. The percentage of hyperventilators identified by Bonn et al. (1984) among candidate
patients with symptoms of hyperventilation is high compared to that of Hibbert and Chan (1989), who
identified 13 of 21 candidates as hyperventilators. However, their version of the VH test seemed to have
been identical to that of Clark et al. (1985), who required patients to breathe fast and deep for only 2 min.
The diagnosis was given if patients scored 60% or higher on a questionnaire assessing similarity between
hyperventilation and panic experience, or 50% if both situations were identical in terms of the symptom
pattern albeit different in intensity.
Although using VH as a way of diagnosing the subgroup of individuals with PD who will respond to
BT is intuitively appealing, it currently lacks empirical support for either its validity or its ability to
predict the results of breathing training. First, if self-reports of hyperventilation symptoms are unrelated
to actual physiological hyperventilation, then such symptom reports are irrelevant for BT. One study of
HVS patients found self-report and physiological hyperventilation to be poorly correlated. Hornsveld,
Garssen, Fiedeldij Dop, van Spiegel, and de Haes (1996) subjected patients with HVS symptoms to two
hyperventilation procedures: one hypocapnic, lowering pCO2 by 50% of the baseline level, and one
isocapnic, keeping pCO2 levels constant by addition of CO2 to the inspired air. Tests were administered
double blind in random order. The authors found that patients were unable to distinguish between the
two tests: symptoms elicited by the isocapnic VH were reported by almost half of the patients as
symptoms occurring in their everyday life (dsymptom recognitionT). Another 18% of the patients did not
recognize their typical symptoms during either test. These results cast doubt on the validity of a VH test
for diagnosing HVS and the validity of the HVS itself, if we accept the methodological claim that
manual titration of 100% CO2 was fast enough and accurate enough to have prevented hypocapnic dips
that could have triggered symptoms. One explanation of the results of isocapnic VH is that fast breathing
had become a conditioned stimulus able to elicit symptoms that had occurred during previous episodes
of hyperventilation. This experiment should be repeated with individuals diagnosed with PD since some
of the patients tested by Hornsveld et al. may have had other diagnoses.
That initial VH predicts the outcome of BT has yet to be demonstrated. In our own study, described in
more detail below under Outcome Measure (Meuret et al., submitted), we failed to find support for a
relationship between pre-therapy response to a VH and subsequent outcome, even when controlling for
initial levels for anxiety. The partial correlations between anxiety or shortness of breath during VH
recovery and improvement in panic disorder severity, controlling for baseline anxiety, were r(32)=.05
and .11, respectively. However, we did not have patients rate the similarity of their VH-elicited
symptoms to their panic attack symptoms.
If a VH test did predict BT outcome in PD, we would then wonder what incremental validity VH
might have over reports of customary symptoms during panic attacks. We would suppose that BT would
be more effective in patients with more respiratory symptoms. Our own results suggest that a
predominance of respiratory symptoms did not predict better BT outcomes. On the other hand,
294 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

respiratory symptoms did predict better outcomes with tricyclic drug treatment of PD. In one study
(Briggs, Stretch, & Brandon, 1993), the PD subgroup with prominent respiratory symptoms suffered
more spontaneous panic attacks and responded better to imipramine than the subgroup without such
symptoms, who suffered more situational panic attacks and responded better to alprazolam. In another
study by Nardi et al. (2003), a respiratory subgroup showed a faster response to treatment with
nortriptyline than did a non-respiratory subgroup, although overall improvement at posttreatment and
follow-up did not differ between subgroups. This fits with the suffocation alarm model that postulates
that patients with true PD, unlike patients with other anxiety disorders, are most likely to have
respiratory symptoms during their attacks and are helped specifically by tricyclic antidepressants such as
imipramine or nortriptyline (Klein, 1980).
Symptoms or physiological measures during VH might provide better predictive information than
interviews or questionnaires assessing what the patient experienced in past attacks, but in order to test for
incremental validity or to apply VH in other ways, standardization of VH is essential. The lack of a
standard test may be why the percentage of identified hyperventilators varies so much between studies.
The VH protocol of Bonn et al. (1984) is likely to elicit more intense and more varied panic symptoms
than the protocol of Hibbert and Chan (1989), since Bonn et al. (1984) requires panting at a rate over 60
breaths/min because it would bmore closely resemble the typical upper thoracic breathing of the habitual
hyperventilating patientQ (p. 668). The VH test of Hibbert and Chan (1989) required only 2 min of
overbreathing. A critical parameter is likely to be the pCO2 level the patient reaches, yet only in one BT
study were end-tidal pCO2 levels monitored and used as a performance goal (Meuret et al., submitted)
(details of the method are in Wilhelm et al., 2001). Reliance on the most visible behavioral aspect of VH,
breathing rate, is insufficient since patients can compensate for higher rates with reductions in tidal
volume, thus avoiding more dramatic falls in pCO2 (Salkovskis et al., 1986). When tidal volume
approaches respiratory dead space, typically about 160 ml in adults, arterial pCO2 can paradoxically rise
even with very high respiratory rates because there is little gas exchange between the lungs and outside
air. Goals for pCO2 levels and how long they are to be maintained must be established or more fearful
patients will avoid hypocapnia by slower, shallower breathing.
Another aspect that has not been standardized is the assessment of symptoms experienced during the
VH. Typically, intensity ratings for hyperventilation have been combined with some type of similarity
rating between the VH experience and typical panic attacks. Two of the three studies used the same
instruments for this purpose (Clark et al., 1985; Hibbert & Chan, 1989), but their criteria for identifying
hyperventilators from it varied. Self-report criteria used by Bonn et al. (1984) were less standardized and
strict, possibly explaining the high percentage of identified hyperventilators in their pre-treatment
sample.
In order to use VH for diagnosis, the pCO2 level reached is unlikely to be the only parameter that
needs to be taken into account, since symptoms and anxiety may also depend on the change from initial,
pre-test pCO2 levels. Non-hyperventilators are more likely to begin the test with normal pCO2 levels
than panic patients, who have hypocapnic levels (e.g., below 30 mm Hg; Bass & Gardner, 1985).
Normocapnic participants would have to change more to reach a criterion of, say, 20 mm Hg, than would
hypocapnic participants. Thus, if it is change that produces symptoms, VH to a fixed pCO2 criterion
would be a weaker stimulus for patients who began the test with lower pCO2 values. Different initial
values might distort measures of pCO2 recovery from VH as well. Change criteria, such as reduction of
pCO2 to a certain proportion of the initial value (e.g., Gorman et al., 1994), might be better, but data are
lacking.
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 295

The amount of prior information given to the subject about the physiology of the test and its
possible effects is also likely to affect the diagnostic value. Emphasis on the unpleasantness of the
symptoms may amplify anxious expectancy and reactions to the test, while information that is more
neutral may make the symptoms predictable and easier to cope with. There has been little
investigation of such factors, and BT studies show no consensus on this issue. Some authors gave
participants limited descriptions of potential symptoms (Bonn et al., 1984), while others introduced
VH simply as ba diagnostic test,Q without specifying what symptoms might arise (Clark et al., 1985;
Salkovskis et al., 1986).

2.2.2. Education about symptoms


Another use of VH is to teach patients how overbreathing can produce the symptoms of their naturally
occurring panic attacks. This rationale is quite common in BT as seen in Table 2, but the exact
educational approach varies. Franklin (1989) administered the VH test three times with varying body
posture (standing, sitting, and lying) and asked patients to identify the first three symptoms they
experienced. Bonn et al. (1984) asked about similarities between VH symptoms and past panic
symptoms, which when similar taught the patient that hyperventilation could have produced those
symptoms. Hibbert and Chan (1989) used the diagnostic VH in the initial session to inform patients bthat
the provocation of their symptoms voluntarily by overbreathing was inconsistent with their fear that the
symptoms were a sign of physical or mental illnessQ (p. 233). In addition, the demonstration of
hyperventilation-producing symptoms was thought to be a motivator for patients to learn controlled
breathing. In the second week, patients were instructed bto practice overbreathing, substituting the
controlled paceQ (p. 233). The overall goal of these exercises seemed to have been the achievement of
greater voluntary control over breathing.
The educational aims of VH vary according to the theory behind the BT. An approach emphasizing a
central role for hypocapnic breathing in the etiology of panic disorder (Ley, 1985) will use VH to
illustrate hypocapnic mechanisms in action and to teach patients how to counteract hypocapnia. This
rationale can be a powerful agent for change. Bonn et al. (1984) reported that VH helped many patients
to experience for the first time a certain degree of control over their symptoms, giving bimmediate relief
from longstanding feelings of helplessness that are characteristic of agoraphobic patientsQ (p. 668). Since
patients are often unaware of hyperventilating if in fact they do, they might not accept the interpretation
of their symptoms as hyperventilation-produced unless they experience the sometimes dramatic effect of
VH reproducing their panic symptoms.
Cognitive–behavioral theoretical approaches, which emphasize the catastrophic misinterpretation of
bodily symptoms, or symptoms becoming conditioned fear stimuli, use VH to teach a different lesson.
Hypocapnic breathing, with its resulting symptoms, is portrayed as one of several natural, harmless
components of the fight–flight response. This response itself is not a panic attack or a direct cause of
panic attacks: the cause of the panic attack is the meaning placed on bodily changes or conditioned fear
responses to them. Therapy handouts from a cognitive–behaviorally oriented group (Barlow, 1993)
contain detailed information about the physiology and psychology of fear and anxiety, such as bthe
fight–flight response is associated with an increase in the speed and depth of breathing[. . .] the feelings
produced by this increase in breathing can include breathlessness, choking or smothering feeling, and
even pains or tightness in the chestQ (p. 26). Similar materials were given to patients by Craske et al.
(1997) as introductions to the first and second sessions, and as additional information in conjunction
with the hyperventilation exercises in therapy. These investigators had patients hyperventilate in a group
296 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

of three to five patients for 1–1 1/2 min, after which patients sat with their eyes closed and were
instructed to breathe very slowly, pausing at the end of each breath. Patients then discussed what they
had experienced in terms of similarity of symptoms to the physical symptoms of panic attacks. After VH,
cognitive–behavior therapists may ask their clients what thoughts were elicited by overbreathing in an
attempt to identify individual catastrophic cognitions and to demonstrate to the client their relationship to
bodily sensations.

2.2.3. Teaching breathing strategies


VH can be used in therapeutic breathing training to help patients achieve greater control of anxiety
and symptoms elicited by hypocapnia. Patients first hyperventilate and then apply the newly learned
anti-hyperventilation breathing patterns (Franklin, 1989; Hibbert & Chan, 1989). They are told that
controlling their breathing will help control their symptoms and should be applied during exposure or
whenever they are anxious. Franklin (1989) instructed patients to repeat VH at least once a day and to
employ the breathing control techniques as often as needed.
CBT protocols often use VH directly or indirectly to teach breathing strategies, although the protocol
rationale may be education and interoceptive exposure. From an educational standpoint, if patients are
able to trigger and then reduce fearful sensations simply by changing the way they breathe, then such
sensations at other times must be a result of overbreathing and are therefore understandable, predictable,
manageable, and harmless (Barlow, 2002). At the same time, even in the absence of explicit instructions,
patients are being taught that, by breathing in certain ways, they can avoid anxiety. Advice to switch
from thoracic to abdominal breathing to manage anxiety generated by interoceptive exposure during VH
(Craske, 1998) is also a message about how to avoid anxiety at other times. If a new breathing strategy is
reinforced by success, it is likely to be adopted by the patient regardless of the intention of the therapist.
To what degree this occurs with specific CBT protocols is a question for research, one particularly
relevant when the benefits of BT as an additional component of CBT are being tested (Schmidt et al.,
2000).

2.2.4. Interoceptive exposure


VH has been used for interoceptive exposure with the aim of extinguishing fear of symptoms such as
dizziness, shortness of breath, and heart racing by repeatedly having the client induce and experience
these symptoms (Craske et al., 2000; Schmidt et al., 2000). VH is only one of a number of ways of
eliciting feared symptoms: Others are rapidly turning around to induce vertigo, breathing through a
narrow straw to elicit air hunger, and breath holding to induce feelings of suffocation. Exercises
producing symptoms that rank high in patients’ ratings in their similarity to panic symptoms are repeated
within sessions and at home. Therapists sometimes teach strategies of anxiety control to be applied
immediately after anxiety is induced by VH. These strategies may be cognitive or directed to breathing
(Craske, 1998).
From an exposure–extinction perspective, control strategies have the potential of being counter-
therapeutic bsafety aids,Q since they might retard extinction by reducing exposure to the anxiety-
provoking elements of the stimulus. From this perspective, if fear of breathing sensations is a symptom
in panic situations, hyperventilation should be encouraged in these situations, not discouraged. This is
one of the reasons that Craske et al. (1997) and Schmidt et al. (2000) conclude that BT does not
contribute to, and may detract from, other psychological treatments of panic (see Meuret et al., 2003 for
a more thorough discussion of this issue).
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 297

2.2.5. Outcome measure


To our knowledge, VH has been used only twice as an outcome measure (Craske et al., 1997; Meuret
et al., submitted), which is surprising considering the widespread use of VH in behavioral PD treatments.
Regardless of whether a treatment rationale considers hyperventilation itself or the cognitive
misinterpretation of breathing sensations as central to the development of panic, psychological and
physiological reactions to VH are eminently relevant to outcome. In the study of Craske et al. (1997),
patients in both BT and interoceptive exposure groups performed a dbehavioral approach testT before and
after treatment. This test included up to 1 min of overbreathing. Self-reports of anticipatory anxiety
before the test and maximum anxiety during VH were collected. Few procedural details of the test were
given and no statistical analysis, but judged by reported means, anxiety elicited by VH decreased equally
in the two groups from pre- to post-therapy.
In our study, individuals with PD were assigned randomly either to receive immediate capnometry-
assisted BT or to wait 4 weeks for treatment (Meuret et al., submitted). For the VH test, patients were
instructed to breathe deeply to lower and maintain an end-tidal pCO2 of 20 mm Hg using an audiotape of
tones to pace their breathing at a moderate speed of 18 breaths/min, while receiving visual pCO2
feedback from a capnometer. They were not told the purpose of the test nor about hyperventilation-
induced panic beforehand. Panic-related symptoms (following DSM-IV criteria) and anxiety severity
were rated before the VH challenge. After the challenge, patients were given 8 min to recover, after
which they retrospectively rated their anxiety and symptoms for the hyperventilation and recovery
periods. Physiological parameters such as respiration rate and pCO2 were measured continuously. This
procedure was repeated several times over the day (some tests were conducted and recorded at home).
The test battery was repeated after 4 weeks for both the group that had been treated (post-treatment) and
for waiting list controls. The first VH of the post-assessment battery compared to the first VH pre-
assessment battery showed higher pCO2 and lower respiration rates during recovery in the BT group
than in the waiting-list group, even after controlling for changes in baseline due to therapy. On the other
hand, self-reported anxiety and symptoms during recovery decreased equally in both groups from pre- to
post-assessment in spite of differences in pCO2 (Meuret et al., 2002). Thus, a physiological measure
during the VH test recovery normalized with active therapy and distinguished the active group from the
waiting list group, while self-report of anxiety and symptoms for the same period did not. Among
symptoms, only self-reported shortness of breath was reduced after active treatment. The improvement
in physiological recovery from VH of the active treatment group was accompanied by a general and
lasting improvement in panic symptomatology. The rate of improvement of recovery pCO2 at different
points in therapy and at follow-up was correlated with the rate of decline in panic symptomatology and
anxiety sensitivity (Meuret et al., submitted).

3. Reliability and validity of voluntary hyperventilation tests

For two of the potential roles for VH tests, diagnosis and outcome, their reliability and validity
become relevant. Conclusions about these psychometric properties are likely to depend on which aspect
of the tests we are considering. Response to VH includes different measures, which can be
conceptualized as test items. These can be divided into self-report items (e.g., reports of intensity of
induced emotions such as anxiety or tension, reports of symptoms, and cognitive evaluations such as
similarity to previous attacks) and physiological items (e.g., level and variability of pCO2, respiration
298 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

rate and tidal volume, heart rate, and skin conductance). How these different items are related, and
whether they comprise a single factor, are classical concerns of psychometric theory. In any case, the
reliability and validity of individual items need to be established empirically, while the importance of
different items depends on one’s theoretical perspective about anxiety.
In general, a cognitive model emphasizes self-report items since they index cognitions, a
hyperventilation perspective emphasizes respiratory physiological items, and a more general
psychophysiological perspective insists on both, as well as on autonomic measures for a complete
picture of emotional response. Pre-VH physiological measures might seem to be an index of whether the
individual tested hyperventilates habitually, but pre-test baselines are unlikely to be representative in
these patients, whose anticipatory anxiety could start them to hyperventilate even before they are
instructed to. Since hypocapnia and other breathing abnormalities occur during anticipatory anxiety
(Alpers, Wilhelm, & Roth, 2005), pre-measures reflect an uncertain combination of how afraid an
individual is of VH and the individual’s tonic anxiety levels, modulated by individual specificity and
anxiety disorder specificity of the effect of anxiety on respiration.
Self-report measures during VH register how the client reacts to the sensations of hyperventilation,
which is central to the catastrophic cognitions theory of panic. Respiratory sensations and the reactions
to them are the essence of a putative vicious circle amplifying anxiety. According to those theories,
reduction of these reactions through prolonged and repeated exposure to hyperventilation (interoceptive
exposure) is an indication of therapeutic progress, although for that to be true, the reduction needs to
generalize beyond the therapeutic setting. In contrast to self report, respiratory behavior during VH itself
is more an index of compliance with instructions rather than of clinical change, since that behavior is
prescribed by the test instructions.
Measures during the recovery period following VH can be taken to represent how well the individual
copes with hyperventilation once it has occurred, one of the primary goals of BT according to the
hyperventilation model. This model regards the patient’s ability to raise pCO2 levels rapidly back to
normal and maintain normal breathing patterns as signs of genuine clinical improvement. Speed of
recovery is likely to be registered in both self-report and physiological measures, but the latter are more
important from the hyperventilation theory perspective because they are closer to the cause of the
pathological anxiety. From an empirical standpoint, any successful therapy should normalize previously
abnormal test results, and two items repeatedly found to be abnormal in panic disorder are self-reported
anxiety during VH itself (Rapee et al., 1992) and speed of recovery of pCO2 (Maddock & Carter, 1991).
The latter abnormality exhibits some disorder specificity, being slowed in panic disorder but not in social
phobia (Wilhelm et al., 2001).

3.1. Reliability of voluntary hyperventilation tests

A reliable diagnostic test shows test–retest reliability, although if the test is reflecting an emotion such
as anxiety, which can fluctuate rapidly, the interval between test repetitions will have to be short. When
the VH test is prolonged or repeated, self-reported symptoms and distress tend to normalize, suggesting a
lack of test–retest reliability. If the levels of pCO2 achieved during VH are kept constant, this
normalization represents a growing dissociation between physiology and self-report measures. For
example, van den Hout, De Jong, Zandbergen, and Merckelbach (1990) found that when healthy
controls lowered their end-tidal CO2 to 50% of its initial level for 90 min, panic symptoms peaked
initially, but then decreased monotonically, returning to baseline levels in spite of continuation of the
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 299

VH. Maddock and Mateo-Bermudez (1990) administered two consecutive VH challenges to individuals
with PD. While 50% of the patients developed symptoms of panic following the first VH test, only 25%
reported symptoms after the second test 35 min later. Maddock and Carter (1991) concluded: bwhen two
anxiogenic challenges are given in rapid succession, the second one is less powerful (p. 850).Q Such
habituation effects are not specific to this respiratory provocation. Maddock and Carter (1991) explained
the low response rate to VH in the study of Gorman et al. (1988) as habituation across provocations: In
that study a VH challenge was scheduled 15 min after patients had been challenged with 5% CO2
inhalations, which could have diminished the response to VH. The subjective effects of inhaling higher
than normal levels of CO2 also tend to diminish over time (van den Hout, van der Molen, Griez,
Lousberg, & Nansen, 1987).
However, that test results change when the test is repeated, rather than being a flaw, may be an
indication that the test is validly reflecting a rapid reduction in fear of hyperventilation symptoms with
interoceptive exposure, just as specific phobics can rapidly lose their fears when confronted with their
phobic object or situation. In other words, such results may indicate real clinical improvement. This
might have been the case in a study where VH challenges were the sole treatment of patients with
hyperventilation syndrome (Compernolle et al., 1979). A large group (N=106) was seen for two initial
treatment sessions, during the second of which they hyperventilated and then rebreathed into a paper bag
until the symptoms subsided. They were also instructed to use this VH challenge daily to induce panic
symptoms at home. The authors reported that in one third of their patients, hyperventilation panic attacks
disappeared with just two treatment sessions and one or two follow-up visits; the remaining patients
required additional treatment for dysfunctional family interactions.
The fact that responses to VH decline when the procedure is repeated has consequences for its
diagnostic use. The threshold for being identified as a member of the hyperventilator panic group may
need to be set lower and lower. If prior experience of patients with voluntary or provoked
hyperventilation is not controlled for, false conclusions can be drawn. For example, in the study of
Hibbert and Chan (1989), patients were given a VH test to diagnose them as hyperventilators or not. In
the BT group, the test was part of the first therapy session prior to any treatment, while in a comparison
group receiving graded in vivo exposure, the VH test was given the first time after the treatment was
over. Thus, one would expect reduced HV test responsivity in the BT group at the end of treatment
because of previous exposure to it, making a direct comparison of post-treatment HV measures
questionable. Furthermore, exposure to feared situations in the comparison group probably entailed
exposure to hyperventilation elicited by those situations, particularly for the hyperventilators, which
would have resulted in diminished responses to VH over the course of treatment. Thus, the bdiagnosticQ
VH test at the end of therapy could have underestimated the percentage of hyperventilators in the
comparison group when compared to the VH test results of the BT group taken before treatment, biasing
predictions of outcome based on hyperventilating status.
A decline in responses to VH when repeated is consistent with its being an outcome measure if some
effective therapeutic procedure was applied between repetitions or if VH itself is therapeutic. As
mentioned above, reduction may represent the therapeutic effect of interoceptive exposure and
correspond to overall therapeutic improvement, but it also may fail to correspond to improvement, either
because reduction with repetition and with therapy is produced by different mechanisms, or because
extinction of fear does not generalize outside the VH test situation. Failure to correspond to improvement
would mean that VH was invalid as an outcome measure. Conceivably, taking repetition into account
might allow the validity of VH as an index of outcome to emerge. In any case, it is important for authors
300 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

to report how often VH was repeated in the various treatment groups. For example, Schmidt et al. (2000)
compared CBT (including interoceptive exposure) to CBT plus BT, but although VH was part of both
therapies, the authors did not say how often it was repeated in either.

3.2. Validity of voluntary hyperventilation tests

In its role as a diagnostic measure in breathing therapy studies, VH could be validated by a successful
prediction of outcome. In its role as an outcome measure, concurrent validity is desirable—that an item
of a VH test correlates with improvement on other measures of outcome. To a certain extent, clinicians
will agree on what is improvement: when the DSM symptoms that define panic disorder diminish or
disappear, the patient must be considered improved. However, theoretical considerations also play a role
in how improvement is defined and assessed. From a cognitive perspective, if patients report less anxiety
in response to VH after having being taught about effects of overbreathing and its harmlessness, and
after learning strategies for combating irrational thoughts, the therapy has been successful. On the other
hand, from a hyperventilation perspective, cognitive therapy’s cognition-changing educational messages
could have contextual demand characteristics that distort patients’ self-reports in the direction of pseudo-
improvement without altering abnormal breathing. For hyperventilation theory, a good outcome would
be indexed by quick recovery from hyperventilation, which is accurately indexed by change in pCO2
levels. In order to avoid that educational elements would bias self-reports, Meuret et al. (submitted)
excluded any references to the initial VH test during the BT intervention. While the idea of
hyperventilation-induced panic had been mentioned as a rationale for therapy, no explicit connection
between the VH and that rationale was made. From a hyperventilation perspective, even one
beducationalQ VH challenge might bgive immediate relief from longstanding feelings of helplessness that
are characteristic of agoraphobic patientsQ (Bonn et al., 1984, p. 668), but this challenge may leave
untouched habitual respiratory dysfunction triggering attacks.
Currently the evidence for the validity of VH tests for diagnosis and measuring outcome in panic
disorder is meager. As discussed above with regard to the roles of VH tests, our study did not find that
initial VH test results had a predictive ability for improvement in breathing therapy. However, an
important VH test measure, pCO2 during recovery from VH, had changed after treatment in the direction
of normalization, which fit with the rationale of the treatment and was compatible with any of the
theoretical perspectives. Confirmation and extension of such results will have to occur before VH tests
can be generally accepted as valid.

4. Recommendations for standardization

One major problem in research with VH in panic disorder is that no common standard of
performing the test has emerged, making comparisons between studies difficult. Standardization is
needed for instrumentation, test procedures, and reporting of test methods and results. All factors
known to influence the results of a VH test, particularly how often it has been repeated, should be
controlled. The extent of possible standardization will depend on the actual role of VH. While
standardization should be comprehensive and stringent when applied to a test of diagnosis or outcome,
standardization may have to be more flexible for therapeutic applications. Standards ultimately are
justified by showing that they result in a reliable and valid test or in an effective therapeutic
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 301

procedure. As of yet, VH parameters have rarely been investigated systematically, so parameter


recommendations are highly tentative.

4.1. Standardization for diagnosis and outcome measurement

Any VH test worth its name must be standardized on physiological criteria of gas exchange indicating
hypocapnic breathing, most importantly inferred arterial pCO2 levels (Bass & Gardner, 1985). Drawing
on previous studies (Maddock & Carter, 1991; Wilhelm et al., 2001), a target end-tidal pCO2 level of 20
mm Hg seems reasonable. A relative target of 50% reduction from baseline pCO2 is an alternative, with
particular appeal when patients start out with pCO2 below 30 mm Hg. Time held at the target level needs
to be specified: between 2 and 6 min is customary. A post-hyperventilation recovery period may
continue to yield information for as long as 8 min. Hornsveld et al. (1995) compared different durations
and depths of VH in healthy controls. Their findings suggest a minimum test duration of 3 min, with
end-tidal pCO2 decreasing to at least 1.9 kPa (14 mm Hg) or well below 50% of baseline in order to
elicit symptoms. In our experience, 14 mm Hg would require considerable exertion for most subjects to
reach, and if an absolute level is to be specified, 20 mm Hg is more easily attainable and reliably
produces symptoms of hyperventilation in most subjects.
We provide pacing tones with increasing and decreasing pitch signaling inspiration and expiration to
guide the rate of breathing. We also provide feedback based on continuously displayed expiratory pCO2
as to whether the participant should breathe deeper or shallower to reach and maintain the target level. A
rate of at least 18 breaths/min is reasonable. Rates above 30 breaths/min are fatiguing and limit how long
the participant will continue. In addition, very fast inspirations and expirations may be so shallow that
they exceed dead space less than normal breathing, resulting paradoxically in a rise in arterial pCO2
(hypercapnia). Even worse, a measurement of end-tidal pCO2 will in this case indicate erroneously low
pCO2 levels because the dead space air contains a high percentage of room air with very low CO2
concentration. Thus, instructions should require both fast, but not too fast, and deep respiration,
monitored by either respiratory strain gauges or direct observation. End-tidal pCO2 should be measured
with a standard capnometer, and if measured at the nostril, it is only valid if the mouth is kept tightly
closed.
The outcome measures for diagnostic VH should include both self-report of anxiety and symptoms,
and physiological measurements. Self-report instruments ideally contain a set of items that capture the
dimensions of symptom and mood self-report related to VH, while also being sensitive to change.
Cognitions related to the VH, such as the experience of loss of control or catastrophic thoughts (such as
in the Hyperventilation Questionnaire of Rapee & Medoro, 1994), should be assessed. Measures should
be taken before, during, and after the VH. The speed of recovery of respiratory parameters, most
importantly pCO2 (Gorman et al., 1988; Maddock & Carter, 1991), is likely to be important since pCO2
recovery is delayed in panic disorder. Autonomic measures also may be relevant, since skin conductance
and heart rate also have shown delayed recovery (Wilhelm et al., 2001). The usefulness of other temporal
parameters, such as speed of drop in pCO2 as minute ventilation is increased, requires further study.
Also to be controlled are overt and covert messages about the test’s purpose and what kind of
experiences can be expected (e.g., whether hyperventilation can produce panic or be dangerous). While
the actual extent of influence of information and patients’ expectation needs to be studied further,
investigators should strive to keep this variable constant within one study, and report the character of
initial information given to the patient. The setting of the test may also need to be controlled. In a recent
302 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

comparison between a VH challenge in the laboratory and the same challenge outside the laboratory
during ambulatory monitoring, we observed remarkably lower intensity in certain reported symptoms,
including anxiety, in the ambulatory setting (Meuret, Wilhelm, Rothkopf, & Roth, 2002).

4.2. Standardization for educational and therapeutic goals

In therapeutic settings, rigid standards for VH are undesirable. The therapist may want to reach a
certain level of symptom intensity in a patient, but since patients differ in their speed of developing
symptoms of hypocapnia and panic, the therapist will need to tailor VH instructions to the individual
case and tell the patient to stop when the planned intensity of symptoms is reached—if possible with the
help of a standardized rating scale. It is probably desirable for the patient to try breathing at about 18–
22 breaths/min at about two times the patient’s usual tidal volume. This should allow the patient to
reach a low pCO2 within a minute or so and maintain it for several minutes (see Wilhelm et al., 2001).
When equipment for measuring physiological parameters is not available, the instructor should take
extra care that patients breathe both faster and with a substantially higher tidal volume, at least twice the
baseline.
All hyperventilation provocations during treatment sessions and outside them should be documented.
Reports should include duration of the provocation, average breathing rate and volume or estimations of
these parameters, and the level of anxiety and symptoms reached on a standardized scale. Patients should
fill in diary logs for these events. The therapist also needs to assess the level of prior information patients
have about hyperventilation, and patient expectations about VH. Patients with respiratory fears and who
have had panic attacks must be warned that VH can cause distress, but too much reassurance or too
complete descriptions of potential reactions can bias reporting and reduce the therapeutic impact of the
experience.
Although in applied therapeutic settings pCO2 measurements are often perceived as uneconomical
and cumbersome, the equipment will probably become cheaper and easier to use. Expanded clinical
applications of capnometry (Meuret, Ritz, Dahme & Roth, 2004), including its use in emergency tracheal
intubations (Cummins & Hazinski, 2000), should lower its future cost. Physiological monitoring makes
sense for any application or therapy that bases its rationale on a scientific understanding of
hyperventilation. As discussed above, verbal reports of hyperventilation symptoms often fail to
correspond to actual physiological hyperventilation. From a hyperventilation model perspective,
physiological evidence can distinguish temporary pseudo-improvement from real improvement in
breathing. Subjective improvement may be reported before the habitual respiratory perturbations that
may trigger hyperventilation-induced panic are corrected. In any case, regardless of therapy rationale,
monitoring of respiration including pCO2 levels provides essential information to the patient and
therapist about how the patient copes with real hypocapnic challenges.

5. Conclusions

Our review of BT studies in panic disorder has uncovered many uncertainties about the use of VH,
some of which are due to the lack of a common reporting standard for VH procedures. Conceptually, a
number of different roles have been assigned to VH in BT or in control interventions for BT studies—
roles that are sometimes in conflict. Applying VH as a diagnostic tool or as an outcome measure is
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 303

complicated by the reduction of self-reported responses, and possibly also of physiological responses,
when VH is repeated. Test standardization depends ultimately on establishing that when VH is
conducted in a specified way, certain response measures meet basic psychometric criteria of reliability
and validity. Important evidence of validity is the ability of the test to predict which patients will benefit
from which therapy, and whether improvement from a specific therapy will be sustained at follow-up.
We have made some tentative suggestions about plausible parameters for VH in testing and therapy, and
for assessment of responses to it. Specification and recording of physiological respiratory parameters are
as essential as specification and recording of self-report.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Research Grant 56094, the
Department of Veterans Affairs, and the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD).

References

Abelson, J. L., Weg, J. G., Nesse, R. M., & Curtis, G. C. (2001). Persistent respiratory irregularity in patients with panic
disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 588 – 595.
Alpers, G. W., Wilhelm, F. H., & Roth, W. T. (2005). Psychophysiological assessment during exposure in driving phobic
patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 126 – 139.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC7
Author.
Antony, M. M., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Response to hyperventilation and 5.5% CO-sub-2 inhalation of subjects
with types of specific phobia, panic disorder, or no mental disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1089 – 1095.
Barlow, D. H. (Ed.). (1993). Clinical Handbook of Psychological Disorders: A Step-By-Step Treatment Manual (2nd ed.). New
York, NY, USA7 The Guilford Press.
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its Disorders (2nd ed.). New York7 Guilford Press.
Bass, C. (1997). Hyperventilation syndrome: A chimera? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 24, 421 – 426.
Bass, C., & Gardner, W. N. (1985). Respiratory and psychiatric abnormalities in chronic symptomatic hyperventilation. British
Medical Journal (Clinical Research Edition), 290, 1387 – 1390.
Bonn, J. A., Readhead, C. P. A., & Timmons, B. H. (1984). Enhanced behavioral response in agoraphobic patients pretreated
with breathing retraining. Lancet, 2, 665 – 669.
Briggs, A. C., Stretch, D. D., & Brandon, S. (1993). Subtyping of panic disorder by symptom profile. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 163, 201 – 209.
Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461 – 470.
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., & Chalkley, A. J. (1985). Respiratory control as a treatment for panic attacks. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 16, 23 – 30.
Compernolle, T., Hoogduin, K., & Joele, L. (1979). Diagnosis and treatment of the hyperventilation syndrome. Psychosomatics,
20, 612 – 625.
Craske, M. G. (1998). Group Treatment of Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia: Comorbidity Study. Unpublished manual.
Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). Therapist’s Guide for the Mastery of your Anxiety and Panic. University of Albany,
State University of New York: Center of Stress and Anxiety Disorders.
Craske, M. G., Barlow, D. H., & Meadows, E. A. (2000). Mastery of your Anxiety and Panic: Therapist Guide for Anxiety,
Panic, and Agoraphobia (MAP-3). San Antonio, TX7 Graywind/Psychological Corporation.
Craske, M. G., Rowe, M., Lewin, M., & Noriega-Dimitri, R. (1997). Interoceptive exposure versus breathing retraining
within cognitive–behavioural therapy for panic disorder with agoraphobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36,
85 – 99.
304 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

Cummins, R. O., & Hazinski, M. F. (2000). New guidelines on tracheal tube confirmation and prevention of dislodgement.
Circulation, 102(Suppl. I), 380 – 384.
Franklin, J. A. (1989). A 6-year follow-up of the effectiveness of respiratory retraining, in-situ isometric relaxation, and
cognitive modification in the treatment of agoraphobia. Behavior Modification, 13, 139 – 167.
Garssen, B., Buikhuisen, M., & van Dyck, R. (1996). Hyperventilation and panic attacks. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
513 – 518.
Gorman, J. M., Fyer, M. R., Goetz, R., Askanazi, J., Liebowitz, M. R., Fyer, A. J., et al. (1988). Ventilatory physiology of
patients with panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 31 – 39.
Gorman, J. M., Papp, L. A., Coplan, J. D., Martinez, J. M., Lennon, R. N., Goetz, R. R., et al. (1994). Anxiogenic effects of
CO-sub-2 and hyperventilation in patients with panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 547 – 553.
Hibbert, G. A., & Chan, M. (1989). Respiratory control: Its contribution to the treatment of panic attacks: A controlled study.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 232 – 236.
Hibbert, G. A., & Pilsbury, D. (1988). Hyperventilation in panic attacks. Ambulant monitoring of transcutaneous carbon
dioxide. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 76 – 80.
Holt, P. E., & Andrews, G. (1989). Hyperventilation and anxiety in panic disorder, social phobia, GAD and normal controls.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 453 – 460.
Hornsveld, H., Garssen, B., & van Spiegel, P. (1995). Voluntary hyperventilation: The influence of duration and depth on the
development of symptoms. Biological Psychology, 40, 299 – 312.
Hornsveld, H. K., Garssen, B., Fiedeldij Dop, M. J. C., van Spiegel, P., & de Haes, J. C. J. M. (1996). Double-blind placebo-
controlled study of the hyperventilation provocation test and the validity of the hyperventilation syndrome. The Lancet, 348,
152 – 158.
Katzman, M. A., Struzik, L., Vijay, N., Coonerty-Femiano, A., Mahamed, S., & Duffin, J. (2002). Central and peripheral
chemoreflexes in panic disorder. Psychiatry Research, 113, 181 – 192.
Klein, D. F. (1980). Anxiety reconceptualized. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 21, 227 – 411.
Klein, D. F. (1993). False suffocation alarms, spontaneous panics, and related conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50,
306 – 318.
Ley, R. (1985). Blood, breath, and fears: A hyperventilation theory of panic attacks and agoraphobia. Clinical Psychology
Review, 5, 271 – 285.
Lousberg, H., Griez, E., & van den Hout, M. A. (1988). CO2 chemosensitivity in panic disorder. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 77, 214 – 218.
Lum, L. C. (1975). Hyperventilation: The tip and the iceberg. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 19, 375 – 383.
Maddock, R. J., & Carter, C. S. (1991). Hyperventilation-induced panic attacks in panic disorder with agoraphobia. Biological
Psychiatry, 29, 843 – 854.
Maddock, R. J., & Mateo-Bermudez, J. (1990). Elevated serum lactate following hyperventilation during glucose infusion in
panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 27, 411 – 418.
Martinez, J. M., Papp, L. A., Coplan, J. D., Anderson, D. E., Mueller, C. M., Klein, D. F., et al. (1996). Ambulatory monitoring
of respiration in anxiety. Anxiety, 2, 296 – 302.
McNally, R. J., Hornig, C. D., & Donnell, C. D. (1995). Clinical versus nonclinical panic: A test of suffocation false alarm
theory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 127 – 131.
Meuret, A. E., Ritz, T., Dahme, B., & Roth, W. T. (2004). Therapeutic use of ambulatory capnography. In M. Jaffe, J. S.
Gravenstein, & D. Paulus (Eds.), Capnography: Clinical aspects—carbon dioxide over time and volume (pp. 129 – 135).
Cambridge7 Cambridge University Press.
Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., Ritz, T., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Breathing training in panic disorder treatment—useful
intervention or impediment to therapy? Behavior Modification, 27, 731 – 754.
Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., Ritz, T., Roth, W. T. (submitted). Effects of capnometry-assisted breathing therapy on symptoms
and respiration in panic disorder.
Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Respiratory biofeedback-assisted therapy in panic disorder. Behavior
Modification, 25, 584 – 605.
Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., & Roth, W. T. (2002). The Hyperventilation Test—A Therapy Outcome Measure for Panic
Disorder? Poster presented at the 22nd National Conference of the Anxiety Disorders Association of America, Austin, TX,
USA.
A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306 305

Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., & Roth, W. T. (2004). Respiratory feedback for treating panic disorder. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 60, 197 – 207.
Meuret, A.E., Wilhelm, F.H., Rothkopf, M., & Roth, W.T. (2002). Psychophysiological Characteristic of Real-life Panic
Attacks: Are Respiratory Parameters Involved? Poster presented at the International Society for Advancement of
Respiratory Psychophysiology, September 30-2, Washington, USA.
Nardi, A. E., Nascimento, I., Valenca, A. M., Lopes, F. L., Mezzasalma, M. A., Zin, W. A., et al. (2003). Respiratory panic
disorder subtype: Acute and long-term response to nortriptyline, a noradrenergic tricyclic antidepressant. Psychiatry
Research, 15, 283 – 293.
Nardi, A. E., Valenca, A. M., Nascimento, I., & Zin, W. A. (2001). Hyperventilation challenge test in panic disorder and
depression with panic attacks. Psychiatry Research, 105, 57 – 65.
Papp, L. A., Martinez, J. M., Klein, D. F., Coplan, J. D., Norman, R. G., Cole, R., et al. (1997). Respiratory psychophysiology
of panic disorder: Three respiratory challenges in 98 subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1557 – 1565.
Rapee, R. M. (1985). A case of panic disorder treated with breathing retraining. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 16, 63 – 65.
Rapee, R. M., Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Response to hyperventilation and inhalation of 5.5%
carbon dioxide-enriched air across the DSM-III-R anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 538 – 552.
Rapee, R. M., & Medoro, L. (1994). Fear of physical sensations and trait anxiety as mediators of the response to
hyperventilation in nonclinical subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 693 – 699.
Reiss, S., & McNally, R. J. (1985). Expectancy model of fear. In S. Reiss, & R. R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in
behavior therapy (pp. 107 – 121). San Diego, CA7 Academic Press.
Roth, W. T., Wilhelm, F. H., & Trabert, W. (1998). Voluntary breath holding in panic and generalized anxiety disorders.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 671 – 679.
Roth, W. T., Wilhelm, F. H., & Pettit, D. (in press). Are current theories of panic falsifiable? Psychological Bulletin.
Roth, W. T., Wilhelm, F. H., Pettit, D., & Meuret, A. E. (in press). Rescuing the Hyperventilation Theory of Panic: Reply.
Psychological Bulletin.
Salkovskis, P. M., Jones, D. R., & Clark, D. M. (1986). Respiratory control in the treatment of panic attacks: Replication and
extension with concurrent measurement of behaviour and pCO2. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 526 – 532.
Schmidt, N. B., Telch, M. J., & Jaimez, T. L. (1996). Biological challenge manipulation of PCO2 levels: A test of Klein’s (1993)
suffocation alarm theory of panic. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 446 – 454.
Schmidt, N. B., Woolaway-Bickel, K., Trakowski, J., Santiago, H., Storey, J., Koselka, M., et al. (2000). Dismantling
cognitive–behavioral treatment for panic disorder: Questioning the utility of breathing retraining. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 68, 417 – 424.
Smoller, J. W., Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., & Kradin, R. L. (1996). Panic anxiety, dyspnea, and respiratory
disease. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 154, 6 – 17.
Spinhoven, P., Onstein, E. J., Sterk, P. J., & le Haen-Versteijnen, D. (1992). The hyperventilation provocation test in panic
disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 453 – 461.
Stein, M. B., Millar, T. W., Larsen, D. K., & Kryger, M. H. (1995). Irregular breathing patterns during sleep in patients with
panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1168 – 1173.
Telch, M. J., Lucas, J. A., Schmidt, N. B., Hanna, H. H., Jaimez, T. L., & Lucas, R. (1993). Group cognitive behavioral
treatment of panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 279 – 287.
van den Hout, M. A., De Jong, P., Zandbergen, J., & Merckelbach, H. (1990). Waning of panic sensations during prolonged
hyperventilation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 445 – 448.
van den Hout, M. A., Hoekstra, R., Arntz, A., Christiaanse, M., Ranschaert, W., & Schouten, E. (1992). Hyperventilation is not
diagnostically specific to panic patients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 182 – 191.
van den Hout, M. A., van der Molen, G. M., Griez, E., Lousberg, H., & Nansen, A. (1987). Reduction of CO2-induced anxiety
in patients with panic attacks after repeated CO2 exposure. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 788 – 791.
Wilhelm, F. H., Alpers, G. W., Meuret, A. E., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Respiratory pathophysiology of clinical anxiety outside the
laboratory: Assessment of end-tidal pCO2, respiratory pattern variability, and transfer function RSA. In J. Fahrenberg (Ed.),
Progress in ambulatory assessment. Gfttingen7 Hogrefe & Huber.
Wilhelm, F. H., Gerlach, A. L., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Slow recovery from voluntary hyperventilation in panic disorder.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 638 – 649.
306 A.E. Meuret et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 285–306

Wilhelm, F. H., Trabert, W., & Roth, W. T. (2001a). Physiologic instability in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder.
Biological Psychiatry, 49(7), 596 – 605.
Wilhelm, F. H., Trabert, W., & Roth, W. T. (2001b). Characteristics of sighing in panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 49,
606 – 614.
Woods, S. W., Charney, D. S., Loke, J., Goodman, W. K., Redmond, D. E., & Heninger, G. R. (1986). Carbon dioxide
sensitivity in panic anxiety. Ventilatory and anxiogenic response to carbon dioxide in healthy subjects and patients with
panic anxiety before and after alprazolam treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 900 – 909.

You might also like