Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 63

St.

Cloud State University


theRepository at St. Cloud State
Culminating Projects in Community Psychology, Department of Community Psychology, Counseling
Counseling and Family Therapy and Family Therapy

9-2016

The Effect of Group Contingencies on Students'


Behavioral Problems in a Classroom
Ching Christy Lai
St. Cloud State University, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/cpcf_etds

Recommended Citation
Lai, Ching Christy, "The Effect of Group Contingencies on Students' Behavioral Problems in a Classroom" (2016). Culminating
Projects in Community Psychology, Counseling and Family Therapy. 20.
https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/cpcf_etds/20

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Community Psychology, Counseling and Family Therapy at theRepository
at St. Cloud State. It has been accepted for inclusion in Culminating Projects in Community Psychology, Counseling and Family Therapy by an
authorized administrator of theRepository at St. Cloud State. For more information, please contact [email protected].
The Effect of Group Contingencies on Students’

Behavioral Problems in a Classroom

by

Ching Christy Lai

A Thesis

Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

St. Cloud State University

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree

Master of Science in Applied Behavior Analysis

September, 2016

Thesis Committee:
Kimberly Schulze, Chairperson
Eric Rudrud
2

Abstract

Inclusion of students with disabilities in regular classrooms has raised wide interest
from educators (National Center for Learning Disabilities, 2014). Researchers have studied
different contingency systems so that students with disabilities and students that engage in
high rates of disruptive behaviors succeed in regular classrooms (Barrish, Saunders, & Wolf,
1969; Donaldson, Vollmer, Krous, Downs, & Berard, 2011; Greenwood, Hops, Delquadri, &
Guild, 1974). Of the reinforcement contingencies, group reinforcement contingencies are
more commonly used mainly due to their economic feasibility and practicality, and utilization
of the peer group to control and enhance classroom behavior (Litoe & Pumroy, 1975). The
present study aimed to study the effectiveness of independent and interdependent group
contingencies on students’ worksheet responses in a classroom. The present study found that
students with ASD (Autism Spectrum Disorder) maintained similarly high worksheet
responses across both academic subjects and types of group contingencies. The group
contingencies appeared to be equally effective. However, more students preferred the
interdependent group contingency for all sessions during the choice conditions for both
academic subjects.
3

Acknowledgements

I would like to express my utmost gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Kim Schulze, and my

committee member, Dr. Eric Rudrud. Without your time and guidance, this study would not

have gone so smoothly. Thank you for always trying to make our Skype meetings happen

despite our time difference. Your constructive feedback and thoughts have always inspired

me.

I would also like to take this opportunity to thank my agency director and my

supervisor for the Little Learners program at Autism Partnership Hong Kong, Mr. Toby

Mountjoy. Thank you for taking the time to meet with me to provide me with some helpful

inputs and supporting my project.

Thanks to my amazing observers for spending the extra time on reviewing the videos

and recording the data, and, most importantly, the positivity and support that you have given

me.

Without you all, it would be impossible for me to come this far, presenting one of my

proudest work.
4

Table of Contents

Chapter Page

I. Introduction ................................................................................................................ 6

II. Literature Review....................................................................................................... 7

Group Contingencies ........................................................................................... 7

Independent Versus Interdependent Group Contingency .................................... 8

Independent Versus Dependent Versus Interdependent Group Contingency....... 21

Interdependent Group Contingency ..................................................................... 24

III. Statement of Purpose ................................................................................................. 34

IV. Method ....................................................................................................................... 35

Participants and Setting........................................................................................ 35

Materials .............................................................................................................. 35

Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement ........................................ 36

Observer Training ................................................................................................ 37

Design Elements and Experimental Conditions .................................................. 38

Pre-assessment ..................................................................................................... 38

Independent Group Contingency ......................................................................... 39

Interdependent Group Contingency ..................................................................... 40

Best Treatment ..................................................................................................... 40

Choice Condition ................................................................................................. 41

V. Results ........................................................................................................................ 42

Language Arts Worksheet .................................................................................... 42

Mathematics Worksheet ....................................................................................... 43


5

Page

VI. Discussion .................................................................................................................. 44

References .............................................................................................................................. 48

Appendices

A. Figures........................................................................................................................ 51

B. Data Collection Form ................................................................................................. 58

C. Token Board for Interdependent Group Contingency ............................................... 60

D. Example of Language Arts Worksheet ....................................................................... 61

E. Example of Mathematics Worksheet ......................................................................... 62


6

Chapter I: Introduction

Inclusion of students with disabilities in regular classrooms has raised wide interest

from educators (National Center for Learning Disabilities, 2014). The Individuals with

Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) is the nation’s federal education law that ensures children

with disabilities are provided with the necessary and additional educational support by public

schools (National Center for Learning Disabilities, 2014). Researchers have studied different

contingency systems to facilitate students with disabilities and students that engage in high

rates of disruptive behaviors to succeed in regular classrooms (Barrish, Saunders, & Wolf,

1969; Donaldson, Vollmer, Krous, Downs, & Berard, 2011; Greenwood, Hops, Delquadri, &

Guild, 1974). The general advantages of an effective reinforcement contingency are to

decrease inappropriate behavior, increase peer’s acceptance (Nevin, Johnson, & Johnson,

1982; Speltz, Shimamura, & McReynolds, 1982), and facilitate teachers in teaching (Ling &

Barnett, 2013). Of the reinforcement contingencies, group reinforcement contingencies are

more commonly used mainly due to their economic feasibility and practicality, and utilization

of the peer group to control and enhance classroom behavior (Litoe & Pumroy, 1975).
7

Chapter II: Literature Review

Group Contingencies

Three group contingencies are commonly used: independent, dependent, and

interdependent group contingencies (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007).

Independent group contingency. Independent group contingency is a contingency

that is presented to all members of a group, with reinforcement delivered based on an

individual’s performance (Cooper et al., 2007). Note that all of the individual contingencies

included in the following studies refer to individual reinforcement contingency in a group

setting, i.e., independent group contingency. For example, the criterion for receiving a

reinforcer is to get accurate answers of 80% or above on a vocabulary test. The independent

group contingency applies to the entire class. Individuals who meet the criterion get access to

the reinforcer.

Dependent group contingency. Dependent group contingency is a contingency that

is presented to the group, with reinforcement being delivered based on the performance of an

individual or a small group (Cooper et al., 2007). For example, the criterion for receiving a

candy is to turn in the homework on time. The dependent group contingency applies to all

members in the group; however, all members, regardless if they turn in homework on time,

get access to the reinforcer only when the target student in the group turns in homework on

time.

Interdependent group contingency. Interdependent group contingency is an

arrangement in which the whole group has to meet the criterion to get access to reinforcement

(Cooper et al., 2007). For example, the criterion set for watching a movie as a reinforcer is to

go to school on time every day for a month. The interdependent group contingency system
8

applies to the entire class. To get access to the reinforcer, all students in the class have to go

to school on time every day for a month. Comparing to other group contingencies,

interdependent group contingency is the most popular group contingency for a number of

reasons (Ling & Barnett, 2013). First, it promotes cooperation among the whole class in

working together to earn the reinforcement. Second, the procedure requires fewer teachers or

assistants to implement and to deliver the reinforcement, compared to other reinforcement

contingencies (Skinner, Skinner, & Sterling-Turner, 2002). The current literature review

includes studies that were conducted on group contingencies in classroom settings.

Independent Versus Interdependent Group Contingency

Herman and Tramontana (1971) studied the effects of instructions and group versus

individual reinforcement in modifying disruptive group behavior during rest periods. Six

preschool-aged students (four boys and two girls) from a class of nineteen participated in the

study. They were referred due to their high rates of disruptive behaviors. A partial interval

time-sampling technique with 10-second intervals to observe and 5-second intervals to record

was used to measure subjects’ engagement in inappropriate behaviors.

Phase I included the baseline measurement of inappropriate behaviors in both regular

classroom and experimental room, followed by token training and token reinforcement

(individual contingency and group contingency, respectively) in the experimental room. The

baseline condition in regular class represented the regular practice of class activities.

Afterwards, the six subjects were randomly assigned into two groups: individual contingency

group and group contingency group. Baseline condition of the two groups was then

conducted in the experimental room. During the token training and token reinforcement in the

experimental room, students were instructed to pick one of the ten toys being shown. Under
9

the individual contingency, students could earn access to the toy when they filled up their

own bins with ping pong balls. The delivery of ping pong balls was based on the display of

appropriate behavior, yet no explicit instructions were given by the implementer. The same

procedure was applied in the group contingency, except there was only one bin being filled.

Students could choose their own toys when they collectively filled their bin.

Phase I results showed that subjects engaged in high number of intervals of

inappropriate behaviors during baseline with a mean of 18.1/ 20 intervals. After the token

training and token reinforcement, subjects’ inappropriate behaviors slightly decreased, with

high and fluctuating day-to-day variability.

During Phase II, instructions were added to reinforcement in the first condition,

followed by a return to baseline in experimental room (with no instructions and

reinforcement), instructions only in experimental room, and reinforcement and instructions in

the experimental room to examine the effects of instruction on subjects’ inappropriate

behaviors.

Results showed that the instructions added to reinforcement condition produced an

immediate and significant decrease in students’ inappropriate behaviors. There were no

differential effects between the two groups. Instructions without reinforcement produced a

slight decrease in inappropriate behavior under the individual contingency; and no change in

inappropriate behavior under the group contingency.

Phase III was a generalization phase including: baseline in regular class,

reinforcement and instructions in the regular class (with the rest of the class and teacher being

present), and return to baseline in regular class. Results showed that during baseline in

regular class condition after the termination of intervention in experimental room, students’
10

number of intervals of inappropriate behaviors increased gradually. When the reinforcement

and instructions condition was applied in regular class, the intervention package did not

produce as immediate and dramatic decrease in subjects’ inappropriate behaviors as the

experimental room. Subjects’ inappropriate behaviors also returned to pre-treatment level

during the return to baseline in regular class condition.

There were interesting findings from the study. First, instructions/rules of the

reinforcement contingency seemed critical in contributing to the effectiveness of the

intervention. In addition, instructions had to be combined with the use of reinforcement to

produce meaningful change in inappropriate behaviors. Second, there were no differential

effects between the individual contingency group and group contingency group. Third, the

difference in results from experimental condition and regular class condition drew our

attention to the feasibility of both individual and group contingency in a natural classroom

setting. The advantage of having experimental condition prior to regular class condition was

to establish reinforcement contingency more effectively and efficiently, yet the disadvantage

of that was poor generalization. Instead of moving the group from experimental room into

regular class with the presence of the rest of the classmates and teacher, future researchers

should consider implementing the entire intervention in regular class, with the subjects only,

gradually adding in the rest of the class.

Long and Williams (1973) examined the comparative effectiveness of group and

individually contingent free time with inner-city junior high school students during math and

geography classes, using a reversal and multiple baseline design. Eight students (five males

and three females) and their teacher from a seventh-grade classroom participated in the study.

The students were selected based on their high engagement in disruptive behaviors. The
11

dependent variables included appropriate behavior, time-off task, and disruptive behavior. A

time sampling technique was employed to measure the target behaviors. Only one behavior

was measured in each 10-second interval, which was determined by the first occurring

behavior. There were seven experimental phases for math periods: Baseline, Structured

Lessons I, Group Contingent Free Time, Structured Lessons II, Individually Contingent Free

Time, Structured Lessons III, and Points (similar to individually contingent free time

condition, but with no exchange value of points), and eight experimental phases for

geography periods: Baseline I, Baseline II, Structured Lessons I, Group Contingent Free

Time, Structured Lessons II, Individually Contingent Free Time, Structured Lessons II, and

Points. There was a one-hour lunchtime between math and geography lessons. The baseline

condition was conducted to measure the frequency of target behaviors in usual lessons. No

rules were given out explicitly. The experimental phase, structured lessons, referred to the

explicit instruction of the classroom rules, without any programmed consequence. The goal

of the phase was to test if well-planned lesson activities with rules only were effective in

increasing appropriate behaviors. Under the group contingent free time condition, students

began with 18 minutes of free time with a deduction of one minute for each rule infraction.

Under the individually contingent free time condition, the reinforcer was determined by

individual performance through a point accumulation system.

Results showed that students engaged in low levels of appropriate behaviors during

the baseline (31% in math lessons and 29% in geography lessons). The structured lesson

condition did not produce noteworthy changes in appropriate behaviors, indicating that well-

planned lesson activities and rules were not effective in increasing students’ appropriate

behaviors. However, after the introduction of group contingent free time (an average of 80%
12

during math lessons) and individually contingent free time (an average of 76% during math

lessons), appropriate behaviors immediately and dramatically increased; when the procedure

was withdrawn, a sharp decline was observed in students’ desired behaviors (from a mean of

80% to 50% under group contingent free time condition), showing that the application of

group and individual contingency was effective in increasing students’ appropriate behaviors.

There was no significant relative effectiveness between individual contingency and

interdependent group contingency; however, group contingency increased students’

appropriate behaviors slightly higher than individual contingency.

It was interesting to note that the procedure for earning free time was different

between group contingency and individual contingency, with the former using deduction of

points/free time and the latter using accumulation of points/ free time. Future studies could

examine if such variable has an effect on the intervention. In addition, the data collection

procedure in the study was a limitation. By only recording the first identifiable behavior of

each interval, Long and Williams (1973) may have underestimated some behaviors while

overestimating others. Future studies should include data collection on all dependent

variables.

Solomon and Tyne (1979) studied the relative effectiveness of individual and

interdependent group contingency systems in reducing off-task behaviors during a 35- to 40-

minute morning reading period in a first-grade class. A multi-element design (baseline,

individual, and interdependent group contingency) was used to compare the effectiveness of

the two group contingency systems. Twenty first-graders from the same class in a rural lower

middle class school district were recruited as the participants. The dependent variable, off-

task behaviors, included four behaviors: physical contact with peer(s), verbal disruption, out-
13

of seat, and general disturbance. A time sampling interval (15 seconds observing, 5 seconds

recording) was used to measure off- task behaviors. A light cuing device, a bare 50-watt light

bulb was used to provide immediate feedback on student(s)’ engagement in off task behaviors

across all conditions. The light was activated by the observer when student engaged in any of

the off-task behaviors, and remained on as long as one of the students engaged in the target

behaviors, and ceased when the behavior(s) stopped. However, students who engaged in off-

task behaviors were not identified by the light nor the observer.

Under baseline condition, the lighting device was utilized with no programmed

consequence. The difference in terms of the use of the light device between the intervention

conditions and the baseline condition was the programmed consequence. Under the

interdependent group contingency, all students earned access to the reinforcement, i.e., 15-

minutes free time and a choice of unsweetened candy when they turned the light on less than

X times in one day. During the interdependent group contingency, the shifting criteria

schedule was utilized, from 10 off-tasks per day to four off-tasks per day. Under the

individual condition, student(s) who turned the light on less than X times in day could get

access to the same reinforcer that was used in the group contingency condition, based on his/

her own behavior. During the individual contingency, the shifting criteria schedule changed

from six off-tasks per day to two off-tasks per day.

Results showed that all students typically met the reinforcement criteria set for the

individual contingency in all sessions, while there was one session when students did not

meet the reinforcement criteria set for the group contingency. The data indicated that the

students engaged in an average of 0.71 off-task behaviors per minute during the baseline-

reversal condition, 0.14 off-task behaviors per minute during the individual contingency, and
14

0.15 off-task behaviors per minute during the group contingency condition. The study’s

results suggested that both contingency systems were equally effective in decreasing students’

off task behaviors, with an 80% reduction in target behaviors in individual condition and a

79% reduction in target behaviors in the group contingency condition when comparing to the

baseline condition. There was no difference between the effectiveness of the individual and

group contingency.

The use of a lighting device was a convenient procedure for teacher to signal any

undesired behaviors, which was likely to be used after the experiment. In addition, it included

the advantage of maximizing teaching time and less time on reprimanding. Moreover, the use

of shifting criteria was critical in teaching students to understand the reinforcement

contingency and shape higher rates of desired behaviors. Future studies should include this

procedure in the intervention.

Nevin et al. (1982) studied the effects of group and individual contingencies on

academic performance and social relations of students with disabilities. The authors included

four studies in the article. All of the studies examined the relative effectiveness of group

contingencies, individual contingencies, and no contingencies, using variations of A-B-A

designs.

In Study 1, four 6-year-old first graders (two boys and two girls) were the subjects of

the study, using an A-B design. All of them were low achieving students with disabilities in a

classroom of 23 children. The dependent variable of the study was the mastery of letter

names of the alphabet, i.e., the number of accurate responses on a flashcard task. During

baseline (individual contingency), students were instructed to study on their own and seek

help from the teacher if needed, teacher’s praise was delivered based on individual’s
15

performance. During the interdependent group contingency condition, students were

instructed to study with a randomly assigned group of varying abilities, and to seek help from

the group members if needed, teacher’s praise was delivered based on overall performance of

the group. Results showed that the number of accurate responses on flashcard tasks of the

target subjects improved significantly under the interdependent group contingency condition,

comparing to the individual contingency condition.

In Study 2, an A-B-A reversal design was utilized. Eleven seventh-graders (six boys

and five girls) participated in the study. They were all low achievers in math and were

generally reported to be disruptive and “not getting along together” in a group. The

dependent variables were the percentage of subjects meeting the criteria for math

achievement, frequency of good study behaviors (i.e., on-task behaviors), and the social

acceptance of isolated subjects. The criteria being set on math achievement was the

completion of 150 math problems within the time limit (criterion changing from 15 minutes

to 10 minutes). The frequency of good study behaviors was measured by the presence of on-

or off-task behavior. The observer observed one subject for a 30-second intervals at one time,

until they completed collecting data for the whole class. The same procedure was repeated

once this cycle was completed. The social acceptance was measured through questionnaire

after the first baseline period and at the end of the group contingency condition. During the

baseline condition (individual contingency), subjects were instructed to work on their own

and seek help from the teacher if needed; subjects earned points based on their own

individual performance of meeting the criteria and demonstration of good study behaviors.

The points could be exchanged for free-time activities. During the interdependent group

contingency condition, subjects were instructed to work as a group and seek help from
16

teammates when needed; subjects earned points for their group based on the group’s

performance in meeting the criteria and the group’s demonstration of good study behaviors.

Everyone in the group under this condition received the same points.

Results of Study 2 showed that no subjects met the criteria for both the multiplication

tests and division tests during the baseline condition. However, all subjects were able to meet

the criteria for the multiplication tests and division tests by the ninth day and the fourth day

of the group-contingency condition of the interdependent group contingency condition,

respectively. In terms of the frequency of on-task study behavior, subjects’ target behavior

increased from a range of 50-60% to a range of 50-75%, compared to the baseline data. When

the group-contingency condition was withdrawn, the students’ academic achievement and the

frequency of on-task study behaviors decreased and somewhat returned to the baseline level,

suggesting that the group contingency condition was effective in increasing subjects’

academic achievement and on-task study behavior. In terms of social acceptance, in general,

subjects were less identified as being rejected by peers after group-contingency period,

compared to the baseline period.

In Study 3, five (four boys and one girl) of the 16 ninth graders from a classroom

participated in the study. They were selected based on their academic and social limitations.

An A-B-A-B design was used to evaluate the relative effectiveness of interdependent group

contingencies and individual contingencies on rate of correct responses in daily math work

during math period, peer ratings of social acceptability, number of negative social interaction

with classmates in social studies class, and achievement in social studies class. During the

baseline condition/individual condition, students received token exchange points based on

their individual performance; during the group contingency condition, students received
17

token exchange points based on all students reaching criteria (i.e., no less than seven correct

math problem), and the number of correct math problems completed by all students. The

group lost its earned points when it failed to meet the criteria. In addition, there were rules

during the group contingency condition given out by the teacher: helping a group member

who could not complete the math problem correctly was more likely to earn points for the

group as a whole; and to use the last five minutes of each math lesson to help a group

member who got less accurate math problem answers.

Results showed that the rate of students’ accurate math responding improved

significantly in the group contingency condition, with the highest accurate response rate of

42.00, compared to the highest rate of accurate responses (10.00) achieved during the

baseline condition. The rate of correct responding decreased significantly during the

withdrawal of group contingency and the reintroduction of individual contingency (a daily

high of 14.20), and increased significantly when the group contingency was reinstated (a

daily high of 155). The results indicated that the interdependent group contingency was more

effective in increasing students’ correct responding in math problems.

In terms of peer ratings of the social acceptability of each classmate, the number of

negative nominations decreased (from 124 to 43), and the number of positive nominations

increased (from 84 to 92) after the group contingency condition. The number of negative

social interaction with classmates in social studies class was collected during the second

individual contingency and the group contingency conditions. Data showed that 9 in 18

students reported having negative social interaction towards the target students during the

second individual contingency condition and 1 in 20 students reporting having negative social

interaction towards the target students during the group contingency condition. It showed that
18

the number of negative social interaction with classmates decreased after the group

contingency condition.

An evaluation was conducted in social studies class to examine students’ achievement

after the individual contingency and second group contingency conditions. Results suggested

that all five students improved academically, and three of the students improved socially.

In Study 4, 22 first graders (15 girls and 7 boys) participated, with the focus on five of

the subjects, based on their socially rejected situations in class. The study was conducted to

evaluate the effectiveness of individual contingency and interdependent group contingency

on student self- esteem, student social relations, and student preference for learning under

individual or group contingencies. The school year started off with individual contingency

condition/ baseline, followed by a mixture of individual and group contingencies. For the first

two months of the school year, the individual contingency was used. During the initial

individual contingency condition, students were assigned to do their work individually and to

seek help from the teacher if needed. The teacher evaluated the students’ performance based

on their individual work. A mixture of individual and group contingencies was introduced

after the first two months. The individual contingency was used on easy worksheet tasks,

while the group contingency was used on more difficult tasks across different subject areas.

During the group contingency condition, students were randomly assigned into small groups

and were instructed to check each other’s progress and to provide help to each other. Students

were given the same grade based on their overall performance.

Results showed that 19 of the 22 students scored higher on their self- esteem

questionnaire with less than six negative responses, compared to baseline with six or more

negative responses. All but one of the five students was chosen more often to be part of the
19

team after the group contingency condition. All students preferred learning under group-

contingency condition than individual- contingency condition.

Results from all four studies by Nevin et al. (1982) indicated the effectiveness of

interdependent group contingency over individual contingency in improving students’

academic performance and peer acceptance.

There were general limitations in the studies. First, based on the target students’

deficits in social skill area, having them seek help from peers might not be feasible in a

natural setting. More descriptions on this procedure under group contingency condition

should be provided for the use of replication and evaluation. Second, students’ levels of peer

acceptance or dislike reports were mainly done through questionnaires. Since the group

contingency condition included the procedure of the teacher giving rules to the entire class,

emphasizing on helping students who do not get as much accurate responses, the study

purpose might be indirectly disclosed, which potentially affected the subjects in giving

preferable scores on target students’ peer acceptability rate.

Lloyd, Eberhardt, and Drake (1996) examined the effects of two teaching variables:

(a) the effect of group study on individuals’ performance, and (b) the relative effectiveness of

individual and group contingency reinforcement contingencies on students’ Spanish

vocabulary quiz performance.

In Experiment 1, an ABAB reversal design was used to evaluate the effect of group

study. Twenty-seven students (15 girls, and 12 boys) in the first-year Spanish class

participated in the study. The condition of students studying individually with no

programmed consequence on their quiz scores was compared to the condition of students

studying in groups with reinforcement being delivered based on individual performance. The
20

dependent variable was the number of accurate words being translated from Spanish to

English. The independent variable was the group-study and individual-reward contingency

condition. The first phase (A) was a condition of individual study and no reward

contingencies. The second phase (B) was a condition of group study and individual

contingencies. Tangibles and social reinforcement were provided to any students who

achieved nine or more accurate responses for the day’s vocabulary quiz. The conditions were

repeated to examine the intervention effect.

Results showed that students’ scores improved to a mean of 87% during the first

intervention condition, compared to a mean of 70% during the baseline condition. Students’

scores decreased to a mean of 68% after the first withdrawal of the intervention condition,

and increased to a mean of 83% followed by the reintroduction of the intervention condition.

In addition, the average range of scores decreased after the intervention was applied, with 8.7

during baseline, 6.1 during the first intervention condition, and 9.5 during the second

baseline, and 7.2 during the second intervention phase. Lloyd et al. (1996) showed that the

intervention was responsible for the increase in accuracy responses on Spanish quiz scores.

In Experiment 2, Lloyd et al. (1996) studied the comparative effectiveness on

improving Spanish quiz score between the individual reinforcement contingency and the

interdependent group contingency reinforcement contingency within a group study condition,

using an ABCBC reversal design. Seventeen students in a first-year Spanish class were the

subjects. The dependent variable was the same as Experiment 1; the independent variables

were the individual and interdependent group reinforcement contingency. (A) and (B)

conditions were the same as Experiment 1. In the first condition (A), students studied

independently with no programmed consequence. In the second condition (B), students


21

studied in groups and received reinforcement based on individual performance. In the third

condition (C), students studied in groups and received reinforcement based on overall

performance of the groups. Members of the group with the higher score or the members of

both groups who met the mean score of nine or higher earned the reinforcement. Social

validation on the preference of group contingencies was also investigated through anonymous

surveys.

During the baseline condition (A), the mean quiz score was 67%; during the

individual group contingency (B), the mean quiz score increased to 86%; during the

interdependent group contingency (C), the mean quiz score was 91%. The first withdrawal of

the interdependent group contingency and the replication of the individual group contingency

decreased the average quiz score to 81%, and the second replication of interdependent group

contingency increased the mean quiz score to 90%. Results from the study showed that the

interdependent group contingency produced more improvement on Spanish quiz score than

the individual contingency, within a group study condition. Moreover, more than 90% of the

students preferred interdependent group contingency than individual group contingency.

The intervention showed significant effectiveness of the interdependent group

contingency within a group study context; however, the study measured the mean of correct

responses within the groups. The limitation of the procedure could be the failure in

identifying if the intervention was effective in improving the Spanish quiz score performance

in low achieving students, in particular. Individual’s quiz scores could be further examined.

Independent Versus Dependent Versus Interdependent Group Contingency

Speltz et al. (1982) examined the effects of procedural variations in group

contingencies on children’s academic and social behaviors. Twelve students (four girls and
22

eight boys) from a learning disabilities class, ranging from 7- to 10-years-old, participated.

They were chosen based on their severe academic deficiencies. The dependent variables were

the correct responses completed on arithmetic worksheet problems during a 10-min period;

and the social interaction among the participants and the students in class, which were

categorized as “positive,” “neutral,” and “negative” interactions based on verbal and/or

nonverbal behavior. The presence or absence of social interaction among the participants and

the students were measured through a 7-second observe, and 7-second record time sampling

data collection method.

The two baseline conditions were introduced in serial order, followed by the

counterbalanced presentation of four contingencies. The first baseline condition was

conducted to identify four target students who were the lowest performing students; at the

same time, it served to identify the highest performing students, who later were assigned to

the same group with one of the target students. During the second baseline, students were

assigned into four groups of three students, and each group worked alone in an experimental

room. The four contingency conditions included individual contingency, all-member group

contingency (i.e., interdependent group contingency with the delivery of reinforcement based

on average performance of the group), identified responder group contingency (i.e.,

dependent group contingency with the delivery of reinforcement based on a low performing

target student’s performance), and unidentified responder group contingency (with the

delivery of reinforcement based on a randomly selected student’s performance). The

experimenter posted a sign to signal students the condition they were under. Prior to the start

of each condition, task instructions and the target student in each group were announced to

students in the group, except for the unidentified responder group contingency. Under the
23

unidentified responder group contingency, an unidentified responder was randomly selected

in each group after the work period had ended. Students’ preference of the contingency

condition was also evaluated through the use of a 5-point Likert scale on questionnaires on

the final day of each condition.

Results showed that the four target students consistently performed better on

arithmetic problems under the group contingency conditions than baseline conditions. Two of

the target students performed best during the identified responder group contingency

condition, while the other two students performed best during the all- member group

contingency condition. One of the subjects performed well under both conditions. There was

no significant relative effect for contingencies on the subjects’ mean worksheet scores.

Two of the groups showed consistent increase in positive social interactions under the

identified responder group contingency condition. However, there was no significant finding

on the increase in positive social interactions under any contingencies, as compared to the

baseline condition. Student rating of contingencies showed that students rated the identified

responder group contingency the highest, while the individual contingency the lowest.

Speltz et al. (1982) reported that the different contingencies were equally effective on

improving students’ mean score on arithmetic worksheets. The major limitation of the study

was the measurement of social interaction during group contingencies. Given that students

were instructed to help each other, it manipulated the positive social interaction data.

Therefore, there was no surprise to see that the positive social interaction increased during the

identified responder group contingency. Future studies should evaluate social interaction

among students after the work period, instead of during the work period.
24

Interdependent Group Contingency

Barrish et al. (1969) studied the effectiveness of interdependent group contingency on

students’ disruptive behaviors in a classroom. An experimental analysis, which included both

reversal and multiple baseline design, was used to study the intervention effect. Twenty-four

fourth graders participated in the study, with seven referred by teacher to school principal due

to their high levels of disruptive behaviors. The target behaviors were out-of-seat and talking-

out behavior. The intervention was carried out during math and reading periods. Students

were divided into two teams based on their seating positions. Students were informed to be

taking part in a competition between two teams to achieve reinforcement criteria to get access

to the “natural” reinforcers, e.g., wear victory tags, put a star by each of its members’ names

on the winner’s chart, line up first for lunch, and take part at the end of the day in a 30-min

free time during which the team(s) would have special projects etc. Both teams could win at

the same time. Whenever a member of the team engaged in a disruptive behavior, a mark was

marked under the team; therefore, the individual consequence was shared by all members of

the team. The reinforcement criterion were set based on the number of inappropriate behavior

being marked during the periods, i.e., either team had fewer mark or either or both teams had

five marks or less. There were four corresponding phases: (I) math (baseline), reading

(baseline); (II) math (game1), reading (baseline); (III) math (reversal); reading (game); (IV)

math (game2); reading (game).

Results showed that after the game was applied in math period, there was a sharp

decline in students’ talking-out behavior (from a median interval of 96% to 19%), and out-of-

seat behavior (from 82% to 9%), compared to baseline. During the same phase, when reading

baseline was conducted, students’ level of talking out loud behavior remained at baseline and
25

a slightly lower out of seat behavior. The withdrawal of the game showed a significant

increase to higher or baseline level of disruptive behaviors, indicating the effectiveness of the

game. During the last phase when the game was applied in both periods, both disruptive

behaviors declined and remained low. Both teams won in all games, except on three

occasions.

The results of the study showed that interdependent group contingency was effective

in decreasing students’ disruptive behavior in classroom. It was interesting to find that the use

of natural reinforcers was effective enough to decrease students’ disruptive behavior. The

advantage of using natural reinforcers was that they were more convenient to prepare and

access in a regular classroom setting. Future research directions could include the replication

of the procedure in a special needs classroom, and the relative effectiveness of using

external/back up reinforcers and natural reinforcers, using the same group contingency

system.

Packard (1970) investigated the effect of a group contingency on classroom attention

with the use of a reversal design experiment. Four classrooms (kindergarten: 32 children,

third grade: 34 children, fifth grade: 25 children, sixth grade: 30 children) from an elementary

school, participated. The dependent variable was attention, which could be controlled by

various stimuli (verbal, written, teacher etc.) Attention was measured by cumulating the time

on a timer. The timer was only engaged when every student was attending. A percentage was

calculated by dividing the cumulative time by the total time for the session. The independent

variables included instructions only, and instructions + interdependent group contingency,

respectively. The experimental conditions included Baseline 1, Baseline 2, Instruction Only 1,

Reinforcement 1, Instructions Only 2, Reinforcement 2, and 3. Starting from the second


26

baseline condition, which was conducted after the first baseline, a light that turned on

contingent upon student’s inattentive behavior, was utilized. Teachers explained the rules to

the students prior to the start of the experimental condition, and evaluated their behavior at

the end of the sessions.

Results showed that during baseline, students demonstrated low rates of attention.

During the Baseline 2 condition, where the light was used with no instructions or

programmed consequence, no change in participants’ attention was noted. When instructions

were given explicitly, a temporary increase in attention was shown in some students and for

some classrooms. When instructions and interdependent group contingency was incorporated

in the experimental condition, there was an immediate and dramatic increase in students’

attention. The classes gradually reached to a 75-80% level of time attending to teacher’s

instructions. Evidence showed that interdependent group contingency with explicit

instructions was effective in increasing students’ attention rate.

However, the study had some limitations. Since “classroom attention” was controlled

by various stimuli (verbal, written, the teacher etc.), the stimuli presented in each classroom

posed an effect on students’ attention, which might contribute to the fluctuating and

inconsistent trend of change in participants’ attention across different classrooms. More

information on the teaching materials used, teaching activity set up and task difficulty should

be included in the study for future replication.

Greenwood et al. (1974) compared the effectiveness of three packages of group

contingencies: rules, rules + feedback, and rules + feedback + group and individual

contingencies, in increasing students’ appropriate behavior in three elementary classrooms

during mathematics and reading periods. A multiple baseline design was used to study the
27

effectiveness of the group contingencies in students’ classroom behavior in a first-grade,

second-grade, and third-grade classrooms, respectively. The teachers and students (28

students in first-grade classroom, 20 students in second-grade classroom, and 20 students in

third-grade classroom) of the three classrooms participated. The dependent variables were the

appropriate classroom behavior and teacher social consequences. The teacher served as the

primary observer of classroom behavior. Teacher social consequences was measured based on

the accuracy of delivery of contingent positive and negative feedback, by the consultant.

There were five conditions in the study: teacher baseline, rules, rules + feedback, package

(rules + feedback + group and individual consequences), and a follow up, which was

conducted 3 weeks after the termination of intervention. Teachers and students selected the

goal together. During the experiment, teacher used a clock light to measure the frequency of

appropriate behavior.

During baseline, the mean levels of appropriate behavior in Classroom A, B, and C

were 47.0%, 60.1%, and 29.7%, respectively. During the rule condition, there was no change

in students’ social appropriate behaviors in all three classrooms. During the rule + feedback

condition, two of the three classrooms showed an increase in students’ appropriate classroom

behavior. During the “package” condition, all three classrooms showed consistent increase in

students’ appropriate classroom behavior. The mean levels of appropriate behaviors were

81.2, 84.6, and 74.2% in classroom A, B, and C, respectively. Similar effects maintained three

weeks after the termination of the intervention program in all three classrooms. In terms of

teacher social consequences, two of the three teachers consistently provided accurate social

consequences; all three teachers consistently provided low to near-zero incorrect social
28

consequences. Teachers’ accuracy in delivering social consequences increased under

consultant instructional and reinforcement control during the “package” condition.

It is important to note that careful consideration on the feasibility of apparatus used to

measure students’ behavior should be made when the teacher him/herself served as the

primary observer. In the study, teachers stopped using Packard’s (1970) clock-light apparatus

after the intervention program was terminated, showing that the social validity of having the

apparatus for behavior measurement might not be high. At the same time, teacher social

consequences could be refined to improve treatment integrity and treatment effectiveness. For

example, ongoing training that includes instructions of procedure and feedback would be

beneficial in a long run, increasing the maintenance effect of the intervention. The accurate

delivery of contingent positive and negative feedback is important, especially during the

initial phase of the intervention.

Ling and Barnett (2013) did a study on increasing preschool student engagement

during group learning activities, i.e., circle time, by using a group contingency. A combined

delayed multiple baseline and reversal design was used to study the effect of interdependent

group contingency procedure used across two preschool classrooms, morning and afternoon

classrooms. Five students from the morning class and two students from the afternoon class

were selected as the participants of the study. They were selected based on their engagement

in high level of disruptive behaviors during circle time through direct observations and

teacher’s interview. The dependent variables included two student variables, disruptions and

engagement; and three instructional variables, teacher positive attention (T-PA), teacher

negative attention (T-NA), and teacher direct instructions (TDI). A 15-second interval time

sampling was used to measure disruptions, T-PA and T-NA. A momentary time sampling on
29

every fifth interval was used to measure students’ engagement and TDI. The teacher used a

counter to keep track of the number of disruptive behavior during the sessions and evaluated

students’ performance at the end of the sessions.

The study started with an initial baseline condition (A1), in which the variables were

measured with teacher using the behavior management procedures typically used in

managing students’ disruptive behavior; followed by the introduction of training and

implementation of interdependent group contingency (B). The group contingency procedure

was then withdrawn (A2) and reinstated (B) to evaluate if the procedure contributed to the

decrease in disruptive behavior and increase in engagement.

Baseline data showed that the implementation of the interdependent group

contingency decreased the percentage of intervals of disruptive behavior immediately

(morning class: from a mean of 72.30% during baseline to 28.57% during intervention;

afternoon class: from a mean of 46% to 9.04%). The morning class met the criteria for

reinforcement during 60% of the intervention sessions, and the afternoon class met the

criteria for reinforcement during 100% of the intervention sessions. In terms of engagement,

the procedure was effective in increasing and maintaining high and stable rates of students’

engagement in the morning class (from a mean of 67.86% to 94.68%) and afternoon class

(from a mean of 76.61% to 98.28%), respectively. In terms of teacher attention, the procedure

contributed to the slight increase in positive attention (morning class: from a mean of 14.42%

to 16.40%, afternoon class: from a mean of 6.12% to 19.73%) and decrease in negative

attention (morning class: from a mean of 30.7% to 12.04%, afternoon class: from a mean of

11.92% to 5.73%). In terms of teacher directed instruction, the procedure was effective in
30

increasing and maintaining high and stable rate of teaching instructions (morning class: from

a mean of 68.6% to 91.3%, afternoon class: from a mean of 95.0% to 95.54%).

In summary, the procedure was effective in decreasing students’ disruptive behaviors,

increasing students’ engagement, and teacher directed instructions. However, the study had

limitations. Results of the study showed that although the intervention procedure was

effective in decreasing disruptive behavior, the rates of disruptive behavior was still high in

general during circle time activities; that showed that the procedure might not be achieving

the purpose of the study to a certain extent. Procedures or phases of the intervention could be

further broken down into smaller steps. For example, start off with the use of interdependent

group contingency in a smaller group or including fewer number of students with disruptive

behaviors in a group to provide more success rate and attention to the selected students before

moving on using the group contingency with the entire class.

Donaldson et al. (2011) evaluated the Good Behavior Game (GBG) in kindergarten

classrooms by using a nonconcurrent multiple baseline design across classrooms. The GBG

was an interdependent group contingency system that included procedures of dividing the

class into teams, creating simple rules, and arranging contingencies for breaking or following

those rules (Donaldson et al., 2011). Ninety-eight students from five kindergarten classrooms

and five teachers participated in the study. The teachers served as the implementers of the

procedure, as well as the observer role. The dependent variables included target responses:

out of seat, talking out of turn, and touching another student. The target responses were

measured through a computer program designed for frequency data collection in real time.

The independent variable, the GBG, was carried out by reminding students of the game rules

and reinforcement criterion prior to the start of the game. For every occurrence of one of the
31

three disruptive behaviors, one hatch mark would be marked on the board, with teachers’

announcing the violation of rule. Whichever team with fewer hatch marks won the game, or

both teams won the game if they met the reinforcement criterion. The reinforcement criterion

was set at least 80% reduction of disruptive behaviors from baseline.

Results showed that compared to the high level of disruptive behaviors measured

during the baseline condition (M=13,5,4,7, and 8 responses per minute in each classroom,

respectively), disruptive behaviors decreased in all classrooms after the game was introduced

(M= 2,1,1,2, and 1 responses per minute, respectively).

In addition to the limitation of not including changes in each individual’s behaviors in

the study, future researchers should make adjustments to increase treatment integrity. The

treatment integrity averaged 60% across all classrooms, which was not very high. That may

explain why disruptive behaviors still maintained after intervention. Although interdependent

group contingency was comparatively easier to implement and was less labor intensive,

longer training and concurrent feedback should be included in the training phase to maximize

the treatment effect.

Ling, Hawkins, and Weber (2011) studied the effects of a class wide interdependent

group contingency designed to improve the behavior of an at-risk student during group

academic activities with an ABAB withdrawal design. The student participant was an 8-year-

old boy, who was referred due to his high levels of off-task behaviors in a first-grade

classroom. The dependent variables included engagement (active and passive) and off-task

behavior (motor and verbal). A 15-s momentary time sampling technique was used to

measure the dependent variables. The intervention included the use of three smiley face

stickers. All students, including the target student, had to demonstrate sitting nicely and
32

attending behaviors to earn each smiley face. Upon the completion of the three smiley faces,

the class was reinforced at the end of the group activities. Negative feedback was given to the

whole class when inappropriate behavior was observed.

Results showed that student engagement increased from a range of 45.80% to 73% of

intervals during baseline to above 80% after the introduction of intervention, and returned to

baseline level during the first withdrawal of the intervention. Student engagement increased

again after the reinstatement of the procedure, proving that the interdependent group

contingency was effective in increasing student engagement. The average of engagement in

the target student was 86.70% across both interventions. In terms of off-task behavior, the

target student engaged in high rates of off-task behaviors during baseline condition (M=56%).

After the first introduction of procedure, off-task behaviors of the target student decreased

immediately. The off-task behaviors increased when the system was withdrawn, yet, were

lower than the initial baseline. They decreased again after the procedure was reintroduced.

The average of student’s off task behaviors was 25% across both interventions.

Although the interdependent group contingency in the study had an immediate effect

on the target students’ behavior, the long- term goal of the intervention was a concern. The

system required the whole class to earn three smiley faces to get access to the reinforcement.

Assuming the system was working smoothly, the reinforcement schedule should be quite

dense that the teacher might not be able to spend much time on teaching. Further, when the

whole class got frequent access to the only reinforcer, i.e., the beanie dolls, the chance of

satiation was quite high. Future studies should include a phase progression. For example,

moving on from initially earning three smiley faces to five smiley faces, gradually and

systematically delaying the reinforcement upon students’ success in earning the smiley faces.
33

In addition, the implementers should conduct preference assessments from time to time to

maintain the motivation of the class.

All in all, significant evidence showed that a group reinforcement contingency was

effective in changing students’ behavior in a classroom setting (Speltz et al., 1982).

According to Litoe and Pumroy (1975), 7 out of 14 studies indicated no significant difference

in the effectiveness between independent group contingencies (or individual contingency in

group setting) and interdependent group contingencies. Interdependent group contingency

system in particular, was effective in decreasing students’ inappropriate behaviors in

classroom settings (Donaldson et al., 2011). In addition, results showed that it was effective

in improving academic performance (Lloyd et al., 1996), appropriate behaviors (Ling et al.,

2011), and social acceptance (Nevin et al., 1982) etc.

Greenwood et al. (1979) investigated the social validation on the Program for

Academic Survival Skills (PASS), which was an interdependent group contingency system.

Results from follow-up questionnaires indicated that an average of 60% of teachers continued

using the procedure in a long run. It suggested that the interdependent group contingency had

high social validity. Based on the evidence and advantages, interdependent group

contingency is a feasible, effective, and preferred procedure in conducting behavior

management in a regular classroom setting.


34

Chapter III: Statement of Purpose

Group reinforcement contingency system has been widely used across classrooms to

decrease students’ inappropriate behaviors, and to increase students’ appropriate behaviors

(Herman & Tramontana, 1971; Long & Williams, 1973). Evidence shows that an

interdependent group contingency is as equally effective as an independent group

contingency (Herman & Tramontana, 1971). To date, group reinforcement contingencies have

been applied to typically-developing students (Solomon & Tyne, 1979), and students with

disabilities (Nevin et al., 1982). The language repertoire of these individuals allowed

including instructions/ rules in addition to the reinforcement contingency. In addition, the

schedule of reinforcement was sparse, generally at the end of the session. However, these

procedures may not be applicable to individuals with ASD due to their social and language

impairment (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). No related study with individuals with

ASD in mainstream school was located.

The purpose of the study was to evaluate the feasibility and effectiveness of utilizing

an interdependent group contingency on changing the behavior of 5-year-olds with ASD in a

setting that simulated a regular classroom setting. The procedure was adapted for the children

with ASD by teaching them to understand the interdependent group contingency by providing

reinforcement contingent on the demonstration of completion of the independent work by all

students in the group. Although the children were not in a mainstream classroom, this study

might show the potential of using an interdependent group contingency with children with

ASD in a regular classroom setting.


35

Chapter IV: Method

Participants and Setting

The study was conducted at Autism Partnership, Hong Kong, an agency that provides

behavioral therapy service for families with children with Autism Spectrum Disorder based

on the principles of Applied Behavior Analysis. Six children participated. They ranged from

3- to 6-years-old and were diagnosed with ASD. One of the students dropped out of the study

before the choice condition due to termination of service.

They study was conducted in Little Learners, which was a classroom simulated group

session that took place from Monday to Friday (1:00 p.m.-3:15 p.m.). The session room was

approximately 4m x 7m and contained two desks, fourteen chairs, two shelves of teaching

materials (e.g., books, arts and crafts materials, toys), a trash bin, and two carpets

(approximately 1m x 2m, and 0.8m x 0.8m, respectively). The intervention was conducted at

Busy Bee Corner at the same time every day for approximately 15 minutes in total. During

independent work time, the therapist provided a worksheet to the students and asked them to

complete it. All children used an individual reinforcement contingency in one-on-one

sessions and an independent group contingency in Little Learners. None of the participants

experienced the use of interdependent group contingency prior to the intervention. The author

and the other therapist at Little Learners was the implementer. All sessions were videotaped

and reviewed by the observer later for data collection. A trained therapist, other than the

author, also served as an observer.

Materials

A video camera was used to record all baseline and training sessions. Worksheets, as

seen in Appendices D and E, were used to measure the percentage of worksheet responses.
36

The length of each worksheet corresponded with how many responses the student could

complete in 5 minutes. A token board, as shown in Appendix C, was used during the

interdependent group contingency condition. Reinforcers (i.e., edibles, e.g., candies, chips,

and crackers) were used throughout the experimental conditions in the study. A data tracking

sheet, as shown in Appendix B, were used to record the percentage of completed worksheet

responses of all students.

Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement

Finish Work was operationally defined as writing in all the answers for the questions

on the worksheet within the 5-minute time limit, regardless of the accuracy of responses. The

instructional tasks included a worksheet task (e.g., therapist issued the instruction “Everyone,

do your work”). The skills (e.g., counting, tracing line, coloring, etc.) that were required to

complete the worksheet were within the students’ repertoire. Data were collected on the

number of completed responses of each student. The experimenter kept all worksheets. Data

were converted to a percentage by dividing the total number of completed responses by the

total number of questions on the worksheet, and then multiplying by 100.

In addition, the average for all team members’ completion of the worksheet was

computed. Data were converted into a percentage by dividing the total number of completed

responses given by all team members by the total number of all worksheets, and multiplying

by 100. For example, in Team 1, Student A completed 5 questions out of 10 questions,

Student B completed 10 questions out of 10 questions, and Student C completes 2 questions

out of 10 questions. The average in which all team members’ completion on the worksheet

task was calculated as [(5+10+2)/ (10+10+10)] x100= 56.7%.


37

Data on interobserver agreement (IOA) was collected across all phases for 100% of

sessions. The IOA was calculated by dividing the agreements on the number of completed

responses on the worksheet task by the total number of agreements and disagreements, and

multiplying by 100. The mean for interobserver reliability on students’ worksheet responses

was 100%.

Observer Training

Observers were behavioral therapists employed by the agency. They had a minimum

of one- year experience on treatment implementation and treatment data collection. Training

consisted of the author providing an informational meeting about the study that included the

explanation on the role and commitment of a data collector, the operational definition of the

target behavior, the data collection method, and the training process. The author also

reviewed the data tracking forms, role played data collection procedures, and calculated the

percentage of completed responses with the observers individually. The author allowed time

to answer observers’ questions regarding the role of data collector.

The author explained the job duties of being a data collector, followed by elaborating

on the operational definition of “finish working” by providing examples for the completed or

incompleted responses on the worksheet tasks. The author taught the observers how to use

the data collection form. The author provided practice worksheets to the observers and asked

them to indicate if responses were complete or incomplete.

The author reviewed and compared the data with the data sheet that the author

completed prior to the observer training. To be qualified to serve as an observer of the study,

the observer needed to reach a 100% IOA during two consecutive role play sessions, which

was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the total number of agreements and
38

disagreements, and multiplying by 100. The observer had a maximum of two attempts to pass

the training. If the observer did not pass the second attempt, he/ she could not participate in

the data collection procedure of the study.

Design Elements and Experimental Conditions

A counterbalanced design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of independent group

contingency and interdependent group contingency on increasing the target responses of

individuals with autism in a classroom setting. The same group of students worked on both

conditions, the Language Arts worksheet condition and the Mathematics worksheet condition.

There were two sessions every day: students worked on a language worksheet task after they

came into the classroom and completed their school routine, and students worked on a

mathematics worksheet task during the second half of the school day, which was at least 1

hour apart from the first language worksheet task. The study consisted of four experimental

conditions for each academic subject. The conditions for the Language Arts worksheet

included Independent group contingency, Interdependent group contingency, Best treatment,

and Choice Condition. The conditions for the Mathematics worksheet were counterbalanced:

Interdependent group contingency, Interdependent group contingency, Best treatment, and

Choice condition.

Pre-assessment

Prior to the study, a pre-assessment was conducted to show that the skills that were

required to complete the worksheet task were within the participants’ repertoire. The pre-

assessment was carried out by the implementer working with each student individually. The

implementer asked the student to complete a worksheet, in which the skill required was

within the student’s mastered learning curriculum (e.g., colors, matching, counting). The
39

implementer provided a verbal praise for every question that the student completed correctly

and a choice of edible when the five minutes ended. If a student verbally said “I don’t know”

or made any requests for seeking help, the implementer removed that worksheet immediately,

and moved on to the next worksheet. The implementer worked on a second attempt on

another kind of worksheet with the students who could complete the worksheet. For example,

if Student A was not able to do a counting worksheet on the first attempt, the implementer

would work on a coloring worksheet with the student on the second attempt. The procedure

ended when there was at least one kind of worksheet being identified as the mastered

independent work for Language Arts and Mathematics respectively. One worksheet was used

for each academic subject. The number of questions that each student needed to work on

were approximately the same. Pre-assessment information were used to determine the length

of worksheets. In addition, the time limit for both academic subjects was the same, i.e., 5

minutes.

Independent Group Contingency

The same independent group contingency procedure that the Little Learners have

been using was used during this condition. The teacher gave out the worksheets to the

students, and gave the instruction, “Everyone, do your worksheet. If you finish your

worksheet by the time I say “Time’s up,” you get a treat.” When time was up, the teacher

held onto a box that contained 6 choices of edibles and announced the end of the worksheet

task. The choices of edibles were selected based on the reinforcement sampling procedure

used by Autism Partnership Hong Kong. The teacher then evaluated with each student. The

student got access to a choice of the edibles based on their own performance on completing

the worksheet. If a student could not complete the worksheet, the teacher provided mild
40

feedback, telling the student that since he/she did not finish the worksheet, he/she did not get

a treat or he/ she would need to try harder next time. The experimental condition was

implemented until stable data points were collected. Data were collected on the number of

completed questions of each student and the average of the group.

Interdependent Group Contingency

During the interdependent group contingency condition, a token board, as seen in

Appendix C, was used. The teacher gave out the worksheets to the students, and announced

that “Everyone, do your worksheet. If all of you finish your worksheets by the time I say

“Time’s up,” everyone gets a treat.” The teacher clarified with the students by asking if the

students were able to get a treat when there was only one friend finishing the worksheet and

emphasized again on “All students need to finish their work”. When time was up, the teacher

used a token board to evaluate with the students. The teacher put the student’s picture on the

token board if he/she completed the worksheet. All students’ faces needed to be on the token

board for the whole group to get a treat from the teacher. When one or more students did not

complete the worksheet, the teacher informed the students by saying that “Not everyone

finished their worksheet, I cannot give out the treat for you guys. Remember the whole group

needs to finish to get a treat.” The experimental condition was conducted until stable data

points or an increasing trend was identified. Data were collected on the percentage of

completed questions of each student and the average of the group.

Best Treatment

Followed by the previous group contingency, the experimenter implemented the

group contingency that resulted in the best performance by the students.


41

Choice Condition

The study ended with the choice condition, in which students were asked to choose

one of the two group contingencies, independent or interdependent group contingency, before

performing the worksheet task. The contingency implemented was decided by the preference

made by the majority of the students. The implementer asked the student one by one outside

the classroom and prior to each worksheet task if he or she wanted to work on his/ her own or

work with friends on candy jar (i.e., the group tokens) for the (academic subject) worksheet

work. If half of the students chose the independent group contingency, while the other half

choose the interdependent group contingency, the experimenter drew the contingency from a

bag in front of all students, which contained two slips that represent the two contingencies.

The implementer announced the group contingency that would be used prior to each

worksheet task under the choice condition.


42

Chapter V: Results

Language Arts Worksheet

Figures 1 and 2 show the results for individual students for Language Arts and

Mathematics worksheet respectively. Figure 3 (top panel) shows the mean percentage of

completed worksheet responses for Language Arts and Mathematics worksheet across

conditions. The first condition for Language Arts (independent) and Mathematics

(interdependent) was carried out for 13 sessions each. Each of the remaining conditions

consisted of 5 sessions.

During the Language Arts independent group contingency, the mean percentage of

students’ worksheet responses was 93.4% (range 61.9% to 100%) with responses increasing

to 100% during the fourth session and remaining above 90% for all remaining sessions (mean

of last 10 sessions =98.7%). The interdependent group contingency was introduced after the

independent group contingency. The average percentage of students’ completed worksheet

responses for the interdependent group contingency was 100%.

Based on the data, the best treatment condition was the interdependent group

contingency for the Language Arts worksheet. The average percentage of students’ completed

worksheet responses for the best treatment condition was 100%.

After the best treatment condition, one participant (Student 1) dropped out with five

students remaining in the study. During the choice condition, the number of students who

chose the interdependent group contingency was 5, 4, 4, 3, and 4. The interdependent group

contingency was used during all five sessions under the choice condition, with the average

percentage of completed worksheet responses at 100%.


43

Mathematics Worksheet

During the Mathematics interdependent group contingency, the mean percentage of

students’ worksheet responses was 87.3% (range 48.3% to 100%) with responses increasing

to 100% and stable during the seventh session and remaining above 80% for all remaining

sessions (mean of last seven sessions = 97.1%). The independent group contingency was

introduced after the interdependent group contingency. The average percentage of students’

completed worksheet responses for the independent group contingency was 96.8% (range

84.2% to 100%).

Based on the data, the best treatment condition was the independent group

contingency for the Mathematics worksheet. The average percentage of students’ completed

worksheet responses for the best treatment condition was 96.1% (range 83.3% to 100%).

The remaining five students continued to participate during the choice condition.

During the choice condition, the number of students who chose the interdependent group

contingency was 4, 3, 3, 4, and 4. The interdependent group contingency was used during all

five sessions under the choice condition, with the average percentage of completed worksheet

responses at 100%.

In summary, the group contingencies appeared to be equally effective. In general,

students’ average percentage of completed worksheet responses under both conditions were

quite high. However, results from the choice condition showed that the majority of the

students preferred the interdependent group contingency over the independent group

contingency
44

Chapter VI: Discussion

The purpose of the study was to evaluate the effectiveness of group contingencies on

students’ completed worksheet responses. Both reinforcement contingencies alone (i.e.,

without concurrent positive/ negative feedback on correctness of responses, and presentation

of the task again upon negative feedback, which was the common practice in Little Learners)

were effective in maintaining high average percentage of worksheet responses.

Prior to the study, students had been using the independent group contingency, which

may explain its shorter time in obtaining stable points on their worksheet responses. On the

contrary, the interdependent group contingency was novel to all. An additional five sessions

were needed for the students to understand the interdependent group contingency during the

initial Mathematics worksheet condition as compared to the independent group contingency

during the Language Arts worksheet. At the same time, for students who understood the

interdependent group contingency sooner than others, crying was observed upon the

implementer’s announcement of not earning the reinforcers. Data showed that after the

students understood the interdependent contingency, it took fewer sessions for the

experimenter to collect stable data points, e.g., during the implementation of interdependent

group contingency for the second experimental condition for the Language Arts worksheet.

The data showed that most students preferred the interdependent group contingency

over the independent group contingency. In addition, it was interesting to note that some of

the students tried to help the students who were comparatively slower in completing the

worksheet. Students went to their peers’ seats and made comments including, “Go go go!

Faster, faster!” There were a few attempts where one of the students tried to do the work for

another student. However, the implementer stopped him by telling him that each student
45

needed to complete his own work. He changed his strategy by telling the answers to his peers

and cheering for the other students. In addition, during the interdependent group contingency,

it was observed that some students stood up and looked at others’ worksheets and commented

“(Student 1) finished! (Student 2) finished!” and encouraged those who had not finished.

Future studies could include the measurement of both positive and negative social interaction

in addition to worksheet responses.

Despite the high average percentage of students’ completed worksheet responses,

there were limitations. Firstly, due to students’ individual difference on cognitive level and

fine motor skills, the worksheet types, which all students had the repertoire to complete were

limited to only a few (i.e., tracing words, coloring for Language Arts worksheet, and

counting, coloring, cutting and pasting for Mathematics worksheet). That might contribute to

higher completed responses for some of the students due to the easier level of work. Future

research could choose a population with a similar cognitive level, so the group can all work

on the same sets of slightly harder worksheets. Alternatively, the experimenter could choose a

novel type of worksheet for all students. A one-to-one teaching phase could be included.

When all students master the skill to complete a certain type of worksheet, the group

contingencies could be applied to evaluate students’ performance when completing novel

worksheets.

Secondly, the number of worksheet responses required of each student was different.

The number of worksheet responses required was estimated by the number of worksheet

responses that the student completed during the pre-assessment phase. The number of

worksheet responses was slightly more than the number that was observed during the pre-

assessment phase. An alternative method for response measurement for future direction could
46

include a rule, like completing a certain number of worksheet responses to get access to the

reinforcement. Moreover, one of the students had not mastered writing or tracing skills prior

to the study. Therefore, the student attempted all worksheet responses by sticking the

answers, which had Velcro sticker at the back, onto the laminated worksheet. These problems

could be addressed if the population has similar repertoires.

As mentioned earlier, since students were exposed to the independent group

contingency prior to the study, future studies could consider choosing a population, who had

not been exposed to either of the group contingencies.

For generalization and maintenance purposes, the interdependent group contingency

could be applied to different tasks within the group session. Ideally, the more often the

interdependent group contingency can be used across segments, the less labor is required and

the less intrusive the teaching facilitation will be, particularly in a classroom with students

with special needs. Therefore, generalizing the use of the interdependent group reinforcement

contingency across different target behaviors and different segments throughout the session

may show meaningful results on the effectiveness of the reinforcement contingency itself.

In addition, due to students’ previous exposure (except for student 6) to the

independent group contingency prior to the study on the target behaviour (i.e., finish work on

worksheet task), the percentage of completed worksheet responses was quite high. Future

research could examine choosing a target behavior, in which the baseline data may be quite

low, to show more significant results on the effectiveness of both reinforcement

contingencies.

The present study was conducted in a simulated classroom with students with ASD.

The procedure could be replicated in the future to evaluate the effectiveness and preferences
47

of the two group contingencies with children with ASD and typically developing students in a

mainstream classroom.

In conclusion, there were no major differences between the independent and

interdependent group contingency, although acquisition under the interdependent group

contingency took slightly longer. The major advantage of using the interdependent group

contingency included the lower demand of labor, since the therapist did not need to monitor

each student or provide concurrent feedback. Future research could collect data on choice of

procedure for the implementer. It was a feasible reinforcement contingency to be applied in a

group setting and might have a higher chance in helping students transition to learn in a

natural group setting, i.e., mainstream classroom. In addition, the awareness to other’s work

and the spontaneous commenting in helping and encouraging peers were an additional benefit

of the study.
48

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.

Barrish, H. H., Saunders, M., & Wolf, M. M. (1969). Good behavior game: Effects of

individual contingencies for group consequences on disruptive behavior in a

classroom. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2(2), 119-124.

Cooper J. O., Heron T. E., & Heward W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.).

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

Donaldson, J. M., Vollmer, T. R., Krous, T., Downs, S., & Berard, K. P. (2011). An evaluation

of the good behavior game in kindergarten classrooms. Journal of Applied Behavior

Analysis, 44(3), 605-609.

Greenwood, C. R., Hops, H., Delquadri, J., & Guild, J. (1974). Group contingencies for

group consequences in classroom management: A further analysis. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 7(3), 413-425.

Greenwood, C. R., Hops, H., Walker, H. M., Guild, J. J., Stokes, J., Young, K. R., Keleman,

K. S., & Willardson, M. (1979). Standardized classroom management program: Social

validation and replication studies in Utah and Oregon. Journal of Applied Behavior

Analysis, 12(2), 235-253.

Herman, S. H., & Tramontana, J. (1971). Instructions and group versus individual

reinforcement in modifying disruptive group behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior

Analysis, 4(2), 113-119.


49

Ling, S. M., & Barnett, D. W. (2013). Increasing preschool student engagement during group

learning activities using a group contingency. Topics in Early Childhood Special

Education, 33(3), 186-196.

Ling, S., Hawkins, R. O., & Weber, D. (2011). Effects of a class wide interdependent group

contingency designed to improve the behavior of an at-risk student. Journal of

Behavioral Education, 20(2), 103-116.

Litoe, L., & Pumroy, D. K. (1975). A brief review of classroom group-oriented contingencies.

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8(3), 341-347.

Lloyd, J. W., Eberhardt, M. J., & Drake, G. P. (1996). Group versus individual reinforcement

contingencies within the context of group study conditions. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 29(2), 189-200.

Long, J. D., & Williams, R. L. (1973). The comparative effectiveness of group and

individually contingent free time with inner-city junior high school students. Journal

of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(3), 465-474.

National Center for Learning Disabilities. (2014). Individuals with Disabilities Education Act

(IDEA). Retrieved from http://www.ncld.org/action-center/ learn-the-law/individuals-

with-disabilities-education-act-idea/

Nevin, A., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1982). Effects of group and individual

contingencies on academic performance and social relations of special needs students.

Journal of Social Psychology, 116(1), 41.

Packard, R. G. (1970). The control of “classroom attention:” A group contingency for

complex behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3(1), 13-28.


50

Skinner, C. H., Skinner, A. L., & Sterling-Turner, H. E. (2002). Best practices in contingency

management: application of individual and group contingencies in educational setting.

Best Practices in School Psychology IV, 817-830.

Solomon, R., & Tyne, T. F. (1979). A comparison of individual and group contingency

systems in a first-grade class. Psychology in The Schools.

Speltz, M. L., Shimamura, J. W., & McReynolds, W. T. (1982). Procedural variations in

group contingencies: effects on children’s academic and social behaviors. Journal of

Applied Behavior Analysis, 15(4), 533-544.


51

Appendix A: Figures
52
53
54
55

Figure 1. Percentage of completed Language Arts worksheet responses over participants and
experimental conditions.
56

Figure 2. Percentage of completed Mathematics worksheet responses over participants and


experimental conditions.
57

Figure 3. Average percentage of completed worksheet responses over academic subjects and
experimental conditions.
58

Appendix B: Data Collection Form

Procedure: Please collect data on the number of completed questions in each student. Please

put down (number of completed questions/total questions) on the box. Start a new column for

every day.

Operational definition of “finish work:” Student writes the answer for the question on the

worksheet task within the 5-minute time limit, regardless of the accuracy of response.

Language Arts

Experimental condition: Independent/Interdependent

Data

Date

Student 1:

Student 2:

Student 3:

Student 4:

Average:
59

Mathematics
Experimental condition: Independent/Interdependent

Data

Date

Student 1:

Student 2:

Student 3:

Student 4:

Average:
60

Appendix C: Token Board for Interdependent Group Contingency


61

Appendix D: Example of Language Arts Worksheet


62

Appendix E: Example of Mathematics Worksheet

You might also like